2006-Padmanabhapillai-Suppression of early evoked gamma band response in male alcoholics

2006-Padmanabhapillai-Suppression of early evoked gamma band response in male alcoholics
2006-Padmanabhapillai-Suppression of early evoked gamma band response in male alcoholics

Suppression of early evoked gamma band response in male alcoholics

during a visual oddball task

Ajayan Padmanabhapillai,Bernice Porjesz *,Mohini Ranganathan,Kevin A.Jones,David B.Chorlian,Yongqiang Tang,Chella Kamarajan,Madhavi Rangaswamy,

Arthur Stimus,Henri Begleiter

Department of Psychiatry,Neurodynamics Laboratory,SUNY Health Science Center,Brooklyn,NY,USA Received 28December 2004;received in revised form 15February 2005;accepted 3March 2005

Available online 12July 2005

Abstract

We investigated the early evoked gamma frequency band activity in alcoholics (n =122)and normal controls (n =72)during a visual oddball task.A time–frequency representation method was applied to EEG data in order to obtain phase-locked gamma band activity (29–45Hz)and was analyzed within a 0–150ms time window range.Significant reduction of the gamma band response in the frontal region during target stimulus processing was observed in alcoholic compared to control subjects.In contrast,significantly higher gamma band response for the non-target stimulus was observed in alcoholics compared to controls.It is suggested that the reduction in early evoked frontal gamma band response to targets may be associated with frontal lobe dysfunction commonly observed in alcoholics.This perhaps can be characterized by a deficient top-down processing mechanism.D 2005Elsevier B.V .All rights reserved.

Keywords:Gamma activity;EEG;Event-related oscillations;Alcoholism;Visual oddball task

1.Introduction

Gamma rhythms are important functional building blocks of brain electrical activity (Basar-Eroglu et al.,1996b ).Oscillatory activities in the gamma range form a group of signals with common frequency characteristics but that are functionally related to diverse brain processes (Schurmann et al.,1995,1997;Sannita et al.,2001).In humans,gamma activity has been observed in a variety of cognitive tasks involving selective attention,mental arithmetic,multistable visual perception,mental rotation,music perception,soma-tosensory perception,visuo-motor coordination,associative learning,semantic processing,short-term memory,con-scious recollection,object representation and induced visual

illusions (Desmedt and Tomberg,1994;Basar-Eroglu et al.,1996a;Tallon-Baudry et al.,1996,1999;Miltner et al.,1999;Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand,1999;Bhattacharya et al.,2001a,b,2002;Burgess and Ali,2002;Schack et al.,2002;Chen et al.,2003;Fell et al.,2003;Umeno et al.,2003).Event related oscillations (EROs)in the gamma range can be found as phase-locked (evoked)or non-phase locked (induced)to the onset of the experimental stimuli (Tallon-Baudry et al.,1996).

Evoked gamma activity has been observed following auditory (Marshall et al.,1996;Yordanova et al.,1997a,b;Karakas and Basar,1998;Haig et al.,1999,2000a,b;Gurtubay et al.,2001;Kaiser and Lutzenberger,2001;Muller et al.,2001;Palva et al.,2002;Debener et al.,2003;Karakas et al.,2003),visual (Tallon-Baudry et al.,1996;Herrmann et al.,1999;Herrmann and Mecklinger,2000b;Braeutigam et al.,2001;Bottger et al.,2002;Senkowski and Herrmann,2002;Watanabe et al.,2002)or somatosensory stimuli (Desmedt and Tomberg,1994;Salenius et al.,1996).It has been observed at early (0–150ms)and later (200–300ms)

0167-8760/$-see front matter D 2005Elsevier B.V .All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.026

*Corresponding author.Department of Psychiatry,Box 1203,Neuro-dynamics Laboratory,SUNY Downstate Medical Center,450Clarkson Ave.,Brooklyn,NY 11203,USA.Tel.:+17182702024;fax:+17182704081.

E-mail address: bp@https://www.360docs.net/doc/4f3509336.html, (B. Porjesz).International Journal of Psychophysiology 60(2006)15–

26

https://www.360docs.net/doc/4f3509336.html,/locate/ijpsycho

time intervals(Herrmann et al.,1999).The functional significance of the early evoked gamma activity has been attributed to sensory processing(Karakas and Basar,1998; Sannita et al.,1999)and is considered to be influenced by bottom-up as well as top-down information processing (Karakas et al.,2001,2003).However,some researchers have associated this gamma activity to early stimulus selection and attentional processing(Tiitinen et al.,1993, 1997;Debener et al.,2003;Fell et al.,2003).

Abnormalities in gamma band activity have been observed in patients with psychiatric disorders(Lee et al., 2003).Studies in schizophrenic patients have shown abnormalities in amplitude,latency,topography and phase coherence of gamma band responses(Haig et al.,2000a; Green et al.,2003;Spencer et al.,2003).Investigations of other psychiatric disorders have been few in number. However,studies of patients with attention deficit hyper-activity disorder(ADHD),autism,Alzheimer’s dementia and epilepsy patients with psychiatric symptoms have revealed aberrations in gamma band responses(Loring et al.,1985;Le Van Quyen et al.,1997;Grice et al.,2001; Yordanova et al.,2001;Green et al.,2003;Spencer et al., 2003).

Although there is a large literature showing the impact of alcoholism on several components of the event related potential(ERP),studies of event related oscillations(EROs) in alcoholics are few.The most commonly reported ERP deficit in alcoholics is the reduced amplitude of P300for the task relevant target stimuli during oddball tasks(for review, see Porjesz and Begleiter,1996).It has been suggested that ERP components are the end products of specific super-positions of oscillations in various frequency bands,and P300consists primarily of delta and theta oscillations (Basar-Eroglu et al.,1992;Yordanova and Kolev,1996; Basar et al.,1999;Karakas et al.,2000).A significant reduction of evoked delta and theta power has been observed in alcoholics while processing the target stimuli in a visual oddball paradigm,indicating that the reduced P300amplitudes reported in alcoholics are caused by deficits in theta and delta oscillations that underlie P300 (Porjesz and Begleiter,2003;Jones et al.,in preparation).In a recent study,EROs were investigated using a Go/No-Go paradigm in alcohol dependent individuals(Kamarajan et al.,2004).A significant reduction in delta and theta bands was observed in the frontal region of alcoholics,suggestive of deficient inhibitory control and information processing mechanisms.In another study conducted in our laboratory,a reduction in frontal midline theta activity was observed in alcoholics during a mental arithmetic task(Suresh et al.,in preparation).

Acute alcohol administration in normal individuals and investigations of heavy social drinkers has shown that EROs are sensitive to the short-term effects of alcohol.Krause et al.(2002)investigated the effect of alcohol on event related synchronization(ERS)and desynchronization(ERD)of theta and alpha bands during an auditory memory task;administration of alcohol decreased the early-appearing ERS responses during auditory encoding and increased the later-appearing ERD responses during retrieval in the theta and lower alpha frequency ranges indicating that alcohol has disorganizing effects on brain electric oscillatory systems during cognitive processing.In another study,Jaaskelainen et al.(2000)investigated the dose-related impact of alcohol on auditory transient evoked40-Hz responses during a selective attention task.They found that higher doses of alcohol significantly suppressed the early evoked gamma responses in both attended and non-attended conditions.As gamma band responses have been associated with several cognitive functions,the authors concluded that this result indicates an alcohol-induced cognitive deficit.This study demonstrated that cognitive processing associated with gamma band activity is affected by alcohol administration. In a recent study,increased EEG synchronization in theta and gamma bands was observed in heavy social drinkers; this finding was similar across the brain areas for resting EEG,as well as during a mental rehearsal task(De Bruin et al.,2004).

Although there are numerous studies showing electro-physiological aberrations in alcoholism,event related gamma band oscillations have not been investigated.It has been demonstrated that gamma oscillations can be generated by GABAergic interneuronal activity(Whitting-ton et al.,2000b).GABAergic receptors have been implicated in mediating the effects of alcohol in the central nervous system(Davies,2003),suggesting a potential role for gamma oscillations in the electrophysiological processes of the alcoholic brain.Hence,in the present study we examined the early evoked gamma band activity in alcoholics during a visual oddball task,one of the most frequently used methods in ERP research.This task was chosen based on:1)the consistent electrophysiological findings in alcoholics during visual oddball tasks,and2) increased early evoked gamma band activity observed during target stimulus processing in visual oddball tasks in healthy individuals(Stefanics et al.,2004).Based on the evidence of electrophysiological deficits observed during target stimulus processing in alcoholics during visual oddball tasks,we hypothesized that the early evoked gamma activity to the target stimuli would be suppressed in alcoholics when compared to controls.

2.Materials and methods

2.1.Subjects

The participants were194adult males ranging from19to 40years of age.The demographic and clinical character-istics of the sample are presented in Table1.The alcoholic subjects(n=122;mean age=32.33years;S.D.=4.28)were recruited from individuals undergoing treatment in the Short Term Alcohol Treatment Unit,Addictive Disease Hospital,

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–26 16

Kings County Hospital Center,New York.Alcoholic subjects had been detoxified in a30-day treatment program and none of the subjects was in the withdrawal phase.The clinical data were obtained using the Bard/Porjesz adult alcoholism battery,a semi-structured clinical assessment schedule based on DSM IV criteria for the evaluation of clinical details of alcohol dependence and alcohol-related medical problems.The control subjects(n=72;mean age=25.64years;S.D.=4.29)were individuals who responded to newspaper advertisements or notices posted in the SUNY Downstate Medical Center.Control individ-uals were social drinkers with no personal or family history of medical or psychiatric problems,including alcoholism and drug dependence.The control subjects were requested to abstain from alcohol and other central nervous system (CNS)-acting substances for5days prior to testing.

Using a questionnaire,all subjects were screened on their own and their relatives’alcohol/drug use and medical/ psychiatric histories.Subjects with a history of drug usage in the last6months for all drugs except marijuana and cocaine were excluded from the https://www.360docs.net/doc/4f3509336.html,rmed consent was obtained from each individual and they were paid for their services.Exclusion criteria for both groups included major medical conditions or current requirement of medi-cation that could affect the central nervous system(CNS). Breath-analyzer tests were administered prior to recordings, and individuals with nonzero readings were excluded. Experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board(IRB).Due to the gender differences in cognitive and electrophysiological parameters the study was restricted to include only male alcoholics and controls.2.2.Stimuli

The visual oddball paradigm employed in the present study has been previously described(Cohen et al.,1994; Porjesz et al.,1998).It consisted of the presentation of three types of visual stimuli(n=280),60ms duration,subtending a visual angle of2.5-,with an interstimulus interval of1.625s. The rare target stimulus(n=35)was the letter X,to which the subject was required to press a button as quickly as possible; the responding hand was alternated across subjects to counterbalance any laterality effects due to responding. Speed was emphasized,but not at the cost of accuracy.The frequently occurring non-target stimuli(n=210)were squares and the novel stimuli(n=35)consisted of colored geometric polygons that were different on each trial;the subject was not required to respond to the non-target and novel stimuli.The probabilities of occurrences of the trials were0.125for the target trials,0.75for the non-target trials, and0.125for the novel trials.The stimuli were presented pseudorandomly with the constraints that neither targets nor novels could be repeated consecutively.The experiment terminated automatically when a minimum number of artifact-free trials had been acquired for each stimulus category:25target,25novel and150non-target.

2.3.Data recording

The subject was seated comfortably in a dimly lit, temperature regulated,sound-attenuated(Industrial Acous-tics Corp.,Bronx,NY)room.Each subject was fitted with an electrode cap(Electro-Cap Intl.,Inc.,Eaton,OH) containing61electrodes and was asked to focus his eyes on a fixation target centrally displayed on a computer monitor.The nose served as the reference and the forehead as ground.Both vertical and horizontal eye movements were monitored.Electrical activity was amplified10K (Sensorium,Charlotte,VT)and recorded over a bandwidth of0.02–50.0Hz at a sampling rate of256.0Hz.The recording epoch was1500ms long which included a pre-stimulus baseline of184ms.Subjects were requested to avoid blinking their eyes and to sit as still as possible.EEG data were recorded from61channels,as follows:AF1/2, AF7/8,AFz,F1/2,F3/4,F5/6,F7/8,Fz,FP1/2,FPz,FC1/2, FC3/4,FC5/6,FCz,C1/2,C3/4,C5/6,Cz,CP1/2,CP3/4, CPz,P1/2,P3/4,Pz,PO1/2,PO7/8,POz,O1/2,Oz,FT7/8, CP5/6,P5/6,P7/8,T7/8and TP7/8.Both vertical and horizontal eye movements were monitored and ocular artifact rejection(>73.3A V)was performed online.

2.3.1.Data analysis

To obtain reliable estimates of localized power of non-stationary evoked potential time series,we employed the S-transform,a recently developed time–frequency representa-tion method(Chu,1996;Stockwell et al.,1996;Theophanis and Queen,2000).The S-transform is a generalization of the Gabor transform(Gabor,1946)and an extension to the

Table1

Sample characteristics

Variables Alcoholics

(N=122)Controls (N=72)

Age(year)

Mean32.3325.64

S.D. 4.28 4.29

Range20–4019–36

Education(year)

Mean11.6614.58

S.D. 2.04 2.11

Range4–209–18

Age of onset of drinking(year)

Mean15.14NA

S.D. 4.96NA

Range8–34NA

No.of drinking days per month a

Mean20.62 2.47

S.D.9.83 2.82

Range0–350–12

No.of drinks b per drinking day a

Mean10.07 1.68

S.D.7.57 1.93

Range0–300–12

NA=not applicable.

a Data are for the6months prior to treatment in alcoholic group.

b One drink=1shot glass of hard liquor;1glass of wine;1bottle of beer.

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–2617

continuous wavelet transform.The S-transform generates a time–frequency representation (TFR)of a signal by inte-grating the signal at each time point with a series of windowed harmonics of various frequencies as follows:

ST f ;s eT?Z V

àV h t eTj f j ??????2p p e às àt eT2f 22e i 2p f t

d t ;

where h (t )is the signal,f is frequency,s is a translation

parameter,the first exponential is the window function,and the second exponential is the harmonic function.The S-transform TFR is computed by shifting the window function down the signal in time by s across a range of frequencies.The window function is Gaussian with 1/f 2variance and scales in width according to the examined frequency.This inverse dependence of the width of the Gaussian window with frequency provides the frequency-dependent resolution.The amplitude envelope of the complex-valued S-transform TFR is calculated by taking the absolute value )ST(f ,s )).This method has been previously described in a genetic linkage study of delta and theta EROs during the same visual oddball paradigm employed in the present study (Jones et al.,2004).The electrophysiological data used in the analyses were derived from trial-averaged visual oddball event related data for target,non-target and novel cases to obtain the evoked time–frequency representation via the S-transform.Mean values were calculated from the time–frequency represen-tation amplitude envelope within time–frequency regions of interest (TFROI’s)(Lachaux et al.,2003)specified by frequency band ranges and time intervals.Further,a baseline corrected time –frequency representation was obtained by subtracting the average value of the time–frequency representation amplitude envelope in the pre-stimulus time window from the post stimulus time–

frequency representation values.This study focused on TFROI corresponding to the gamma (29–45Hz)frequency band and 0–150ms time window range.Fig.1shows TFROI’s at the F3electrode for the target and non-target stimuli in the control and alcoholic groups.It can be seen in the figure that while the control group manifests higher gamma response for target stimuli,the alcoholic group manifests higher gamma activity for the non-target stimuli.2.4.Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed on the evoked gamma activity obtained during the target,non-target and novel stimulus conditions.Mean energy values of the evoked gamma data (time window 0–150ms)from 9electrodes (F3,F4,Fz,C3,C4,Cz,P3,P4,Pz)were analyzed by a linear mixed-effects model.These 9electrodes were divided into three topographical regions,frontal (F3/F4/Fz),central (C3/C4/Cz)and parietal (P3/P4/Pz).Frontal,central and parietal regions were analyzed separately by linear mixed-effects models using SAS Proc Mixed procedure (SAS 9,SAS Institute Inc.,NC,USA).The mixed-effects model included group (alcoholics,controls),condition (target,non-target and novel),electrodes (within a region)and their interactions as fixed effects.Since the alcoholics were significantly older and less educated than the controls (p <0.001),both age and education were also entered in the model.A backward stepwise method was used to remove non-significant effects.Further exploration of main effects and interactions were performed using Wald’s tests (Kenward and Roger,1997).The behavioral data and clinical characteristics were analyzed using univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOV A)and F t _

tests.

Fig.1.Time –frequency representation of evoked gamma band energy distribution (29–45Hz)of target and non-target stimuli at F3electrode,calculated using the S-transform method.(A)Target stimuli in controls (CTL)and alcoholics (ALC).(B)Non-Target stimuli in controls (CTL)and alcoholics (ALC).The time –frequency region of interest (TFROI)window used for analysis was 0–150ms (White Square).The time –frequency distribution displayed above was obtained by calculating Z -scores within individual frequencies across the two groups of subjects in each stimulus condition (Target and Non-Target)separately.Significant reduction in early evoked gamma band response is observable in alcoholics (ALC)for the target condition.However,an opposite trend is seen in the non-target condition (higher evoked gamma band response in alcoholics).

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology 60(2006)15–26

18

3.Results

3.1.Clinical characteristics

Age and education of alcoholic and control groups were compared using t-tests.Alcoholics were found to be significantly older(t=à10.67;p<0.001)and less educated (t=10.06;p<0.001)than control subjects.3.2.Behavioral data

The mean reaction time of the two groups were compared using ANCOV A with age as a covariate(F1,220=0.643; p=0.424).Although there was no statistically significant difference between the two groups,the control group had slightly faster reaction times(mean=462.05ms;S.D.=77.52) than the alcoholic group(mean=473.20ms;S.D.=

95.48).

B)

Fig.2.The bar diagrams within the head plots show mean gamma energy values obtained through baseline corrected TFR(time–frequency representation) calculated using the S-transform for each of the electrodes:(A)Target and non-target stimuli within control and alcoholic groups;note that the controls manifest increased gamma response to target compared to non-target stimuli,while alcoholics manifest decreased gamma response to target stimuli compared to non-target stimuli.(B)Control and alcoholic groups within target and non-target stimulus conditions.The control group manifests increased gamma response to targets compared to the alcoholic group;alcoholics manifest increased gamma compared to controls for non-target stimuli at most leads particularly over frontal regions.Statistically significant differences were obtained only in the frontal region.

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–2619

Similarly,the mean total error scores (both omission and commission)of the two groups were compared using ANCOV A with age as a covariate (F 1,220=3.12;p =0.079).The alcoholic group had a higher error rate (mean =3.43;S.D.=3.88)than the controls (mean =1.99;S.D.=2.78),but this was not statistically significant.3.3.Gamma band response

The mean gamma energy during target and non-target stimulus conditions in the control and alcoholic groups is illustrated in Fig.2.The control group had higher gamma band response for target stimuli at all electrodes compared to the alcoholic group.For the non-target stimuli,the alcoholic group had higher gamma band response at most of the electrodes.The mean and standard deviation of gamma band response in controls and alcoholics in target and non-target conditions are shown in Table 2.Our analysis indicated statistically significant results only in the frontal region.There were no significant main effects or interaction effects in the central or parietal regions.Hence,in the following section we have reported only the significant results obtained in the frontal region.

Statistical analysis yielded a significant Condition ?Group interaction (F (2,384)=7.10;p <0.001)in the frontal region.The control group obtained significantly higher gamma band response than the alcoholic group in the target condition (Wald t -statistic =2.63,p <0.01).In contrast,the alcoholic group had significantly higher gamma band response for the non-target condition compared to the

control group,indicating differential information process-ing in both groups (Wald t -statistic =à2.51,p <0.05).This differential effect is illustrated in Fig.1.There was no significant difference in gamma band response to novel stimuli between control and alcoholic groups (Wald t -statistic =1.38,p =0.17).Although the alcoholic group was significantly older and less educated than the control group,age and education did not yield statistically significant effects on gamma band response.This indi-cates that the significant group differences in gamma band response obtained during target and non-target stimulus conditions is not due to the effect of age and education variables.

4.Discussion

The present study compared the early evoked gamma band activity in alcoholics and controls during a visual oddball task.The target stimuli elicited increased early evoked gamma band activity in control subjects,and significantly reduced gamma band response in the alco-holic group in the frontal region.In contrast,the non-target stimuli elicited significantly higher gamma band response in alcoholic subjects compared to control subjects.Although there is abundant evidence of frontal lobe dysfunction in alcohol dependent individuals,this is the first study to demonstrate an abnormal gamma band response in alcoholics.

The suppression of gamma band activity to target stimuli observed in the frontal region of alcoholics may be associated with cognitive processes involved in the oddball task used in the present study.This task consisted of visual stimuli that could easily be verbalized.Moreover,it involved stimulus discrimination that necessitated the maintenance of information in working memory.The same neural system that has been found to be activated during working memory tasks in both humans and animals is also involved in the processing of infrequent targets in oddball tasks (Friedman and Goldman-Rakic,1994;McCarthy et al.,1994,1996,1997;Smith and Maes,1995).In addition,a number of event related fMRI studies have shown frontal activation to the target stimuli during the visual oddball task (McCarthy et al.,1997;Yoshiura et al.,1999;Clark et al.,2000;Ardekani et al.,2002).Further evidence comes from a recent study,similar to the present study that explored the structural correlates of target processing in subjects at risk for developing alcoholism during a visual oddball task using fMRI (Rangaswamy et al.,2004).It was concluded that the absence of activation in the inferior frontal gyrus of high risk subjects was associated with their ineffective rehearsal component of working memory.Based on the abundant evidence of executive function deficits in chronic alcoholics (Giancola and Moss,1998),it is possible that the reduction in gamma band response reflects frontal lobe dysfunction in our alcoholic group.

Table 2

Mean and standard deviation of evoked gamma band response in controls and alcoholics during target and non-target condition Electrode Controls Alcoholics Mean S.D.Mean S.D.Target F30.039250.13554à0.002680.14010FZ 0.021670.12740à0.001790.13839F40.023070.143310.000580.14259C30.022300.136380.008770.14190CZ 0.017110.143560.000010.13824C40.026280.138930.004180.13641P30.041180.133550.009070.13913PZ 0.029080.134110.018600.13065P40.04526

0.14226

0.02365

0.13533

Non-target F3à0.002050.105690.030280.12085FZ 0.002890.116150.033040.11423F4à0.0020.118310.021460.11811C30.009860.112980.030660.11594CZ 0.019330.116380.024090.11102C40.010440.118590.010070.12033P30.030850.142450.039390.12545PZ 0.033350.128770.031550.12355P4

0.035720.124660.037060.13306

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology 60(2006)15–26

20

Unlike control subjects,alcoholic subjects had higher gamma band response to the non-target stimuli in the frontal region.This increase in gamma band response to the irrelevant stimuli indicates abnormal information processing and a probable reorganization of functional systems in the alcoholic group.Pfefferbaum et al.(2001),in an fMRI study,found that alcoholics have diminished activation in the frontal cortical systems employed by normal subjects during a spatial working memory task.Alcoholic subjects had higher activation in other regions of the frontal lobes suggesting that alcoholic subjects invoke inappropriate brain systems when engaged in a task requiring visuospatial working memory.Similarly,electrophysiological evidence of working memory deficits in different brain regions including frontal lobes have been reported in alcoholics during spatial working memory tasks(Zhang et al., 1997a,b).It has been proposed that the early evoked gamma band response reflects a matching process between the stimulus held in working memory and the incoming,just perceived stimulus(Herrmann and Mecklinger,2000a; Debener et al.,2003).This template matching in working memory facilitates the discrimination between the target and non-target stimuli.In addition,it has been shown that gamma oscillations play a significant role in working memory processes(Jensen and Lisman,1998;Burle and Bonnet,2000;Elliott and Muller,2000).Hence,it can be assumed that an impaired working memory system in alcoholics leads to an aberrant discriminatory process, resulting in augmented processing of the irrelevant stimuli.

The higher gamma band response for the target stimuli in the frontal region in the control group is consistent with previous reports on visual gamma processing.Increased early evoked gamma activity has been observed in the lateral frontal regions during illusory figure(Kanizsa) perception(Herrmann et al.,1999;Herrmann and Meck-linger,2000a,b).Karakas et al.(2003)found that early evoked gamma was correlated with neuropsychological functions such as attention,learning,memory and executive functions.In contrast,lack of early gamma band response was related to perseverative responses on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test(WCST).As the presence of early gamma band activity itself is related to higher cognitive functions pertaining to frontal lobes,it was suggested that the early evoked gamma band response reflects top-down informa-tion processing.It could be hypothesized that the reduced early evoked gamma activity observed in the alcoholic group in the present study may be due to a deficient top-down information processing mechanism.

Top-down processing occurs in situations when mapping between sensory inputs,thoughts,and actions,are either weakly established or are rapidly changing(Miller and Cohen,2001).The prefrontal cortex plays a fundamental role in such situations in which the behavior must be guided by internal states or intentions(Cohen and Servan-Schreiber,1992;Miller,2000).According to Engel et al. (2001),top-down influences could be defined as intrinsic sources of contextual modulation of neural processing, which includes the activity of systems involved in goal definition,action planning,working memory and selective attention.Prefrontal cortex plays a major role in mediating these functions(Fuster,1989;Desimone and Duncan,1995; Kastner and Ungerleider,2000;Schultz,2000;Schall, 2001).It has been suggested that assemblies of neurons that represent action goals in the prefrontal cortex provide modulatory F bias signals_to the sensory motor circuits that have to carry out response selection(Miller,2000;Miller and Cohen,2001).This process is executed through the extensive interconnections of prefrontal cortex with other neocortical regions,inclusive of all the sensory systems, cortical and subcortical motor system structures,and also with limbic and midbrain structures involved in affect, memory and reward(Stuss and Benson,1986;Fuster, 1989).Several studies have suggested the involvement of prefrontal cortex in exerting a top-down influence on visual processing and visual object recognition(Miyashita and Hayashi,2000;Corbetta and Shulman,2002;Bar,2003). Moreover,damage to prefrontal cortex results in disinhibi-tion of input to primary cortical regions(Skinner and Yingling,1976;Knight et al.,1989,1999;Yamaguchi and Knight,1990),indicating its role in the early selection of sensory inputs.

Chronic alcoholism has been linked to a wide range of structural and functional abnormalities in frontal lobes(for review,see Moselhy et al.,2001).Several studies have reported neuropsychological and frontal executive function deficits in alcohol dependent individuals(Jones,1971; Jones and Parsons,1972;Tarter,1973;Acker,1985; Wilkinson and Poulos,1987;Sullivan et al.,1993,2002; Beatty et al.,1996;Nixon and Bowlby,1996;Ratti et al., 2002).Neuroimaging studies have shown that the executive function deficits of alcohol dependent individuals may be associated with decreased frontal glucose metabolism (Adams et al.,1993;Wang et al.,1993;Gansler et al., 2000)and regional cerebral blood flow in frontal lobes of alcoholics(O’Carroll et al.,1991;Nicolas et al.,1993; Gansler et al.,2000).Furthermore,postmortem and neuro-radiological studies have revealed cortical atrophy and reduction in grey matter and white matter in the frontal lobes of alcoholics(Harper et al.,1985;Pfefferbaum et al., 1997).Evidence from electrophysiological studies have shown frontal abnormalities in alcoholics(Begleiter et al., 1980;Porjesz and Begleiter,1987;Michael et al.,1993; Rodriguez-Holguin et al.,1999;Hada et al.,2000; Kamarajan et al.,2004).The findings of the present study are consistent with the notion of frontal lobe dysfunction observed in alcoholics.The putative mechanisms underlying these deficits could possibly be related to the biological underpinnings of gamma band activity.

Gamma oscillations are inhibitory rhythms involving GABAergic interneuronal activity(Whittington et al., 2000b).Gamma oscillations have been observed in different areas of the brain including hippocampus,sensory motor

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–2621

areas,auditory and visual cortices and also in thalamocort-ical systems(Whittington et al.,2000a).Local or short range gamma synchrony is thought to be generated when metabotropic glutamate receptors activate GABAergic interneurons(GABA A receptor mediated)leading to an ongoing mutual inhibition of post synaptic interneuron potentials.The synchronous mutual inhibition of the interneurons produces a recurrent feedback loop which forms the basis for the generation of gamma activity (Whittington et al.,1995;Traub et al.,1996).GABA A receptor antagonists such as bicuculline or picrotoxin have been shown to abolish gamma activity(Stopfer et al.,1997; Whittington et al.,1997;Colling et al.,1998)supporting a role for GABA A receptor mediation in the generation of synchronous gamma oscillations.

GABA A receptors occupy a central role in mediating the effects of ethanol in the central nervous system(CNS) (Davies,2003).Several molecular,cellular and behavioral studies have revealed that GABA A receptors are involved in the acute actions of ethanol,ethanol intolerance, ethanol dependence and ethanol self administration (Grobin et al.,1998).Moreover,GABA A receptor activation mediates many of the behavioral and motiva-tional effects of ethanol(Chester and Cunningham,2002). It has been suggested that chronic alcoholism damages neurons containing GABA A/Benzodiazepine receptors in the frontal region(Gilman et al.,1996;Lingford-Hughes et al.,1998).Differential expression and distribution of certain subunits of GABA A receptor in the superior frontal cortex of human alcoholic brain have also been observed(Buckley et al.,2000).Another fast oscillation, namely beta,also mediated by GABAergic mechanisms has been shown to have linkage with a GABA A receptor gene(Porjesz et al.,2002).In a recent study,the same GABA A receptor gene(alpha2)was found to be associated with both beta oscillations and alcohol depend-ence(Edenberg et al.,2004).It is possible that the reduction in gamma band activity in alcoholics observed during target processing in the present study is associated with dysfunction of GABAergic mechanisms.Moreover, there is evidence that changes in GABA A receptors reflect processes underlying spatial working memory in the prefrontal cortex(Rao et al.,2000).This supports the findings of the present study,since the oddball task also involves these cognitive components.

In conclusion,the present study demonstrates deficient early evoked gamma band activity in alcohol dependent individuals.This deficit was observed in the frontal region which is consistent with the widely reported frontal brain dysfunction in alcoholics.In addition,these findings suggest a basic deficit in GABAergic mechanisms in alcoholics, given the role of GABAergic interneurons in generating and modulating gamma band activity,as well as the role of GABA in the effects of alcohol on the brain(e.g.tolerance, physical dependence,incoordination).Studies are underway in high risk individuals to determine whether this gamma deficit observed in alcoholics in the present study is a F trait_ or F state_variable.

Acknowledgments

We thank Aquanette Sass,Aleksey Dumer,Lakshmi Krishnamurthy,Glenn Murawski,Tracy Crippen,Carlene Haynes,and Joyce Alonzia for their valuable technical support.This study was supported by the NIH grant#5RO1 AA02686from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism(NIAAA).

References

Acker,C.,1985.Performance of female alcoholics on neuropsychological testing.Alcohol Alcohol.20,379–386.

Adams,K.M.,Gilman,S.,Koeppe,R.A.,Kluin,K.J.,Brunberg,J.A.,Dede,

D.,Berent,S.,Kroll,P.D.,1993.Neuropsychological deficits are

correlated with frontal hypometabolism in positron emission tomog-raphy studies of older alcoholic patients.Alcohol.,Clin.Exp.Res.17, 205–210.

Ardekani,B.A.,Choi,S.J.,Hossein-Zadeh,G.A.,Porjesz,B.,Tanabe,J.L., Lim,K.O.,Bilder,R.,Helpern,J.A.,Begleiter,H.,2002.Functional magnetic resonance imaging of brain activity in the visual oddball task.

Brain Res.Cogn.Brain Res.14,347–356.

Bar,M.,2003.A cortical mechanism for triggering top-down facilitation in visual object recognition.J.Cogn.Neurosci.15,600–609.

Basar, E.,Basar-Eroglu, C.,Karakas,S.,Schurmann,M.,1999.Are cognitive processes manifested in event-related gamma,alpha,theta and delta oscillations in the EEG?Neurosci.Lett.259,165–168.

Basar-Eroglu, C.,Basar, E.,Demiralp,T.,Schurmann,M.,1992.

P300-response:possible psychophysiological correlates in delta and theta frequency channels.A review.Int.J.Psychophysiol.13, 161–179.

Basar-Eroglu,C.,Struber,D.,Kruse,P.,Basar,E.,Stadler,M.,1996a.

Frontal gamma-band enhancement during multistable visual perception.

Int.J.Psychophysiol.24,113–125.

Basar-Eroglu, C.,Struber, D.,Schurmann,M.,Stadler,M.,Basar, E., 1996b.Gamma-band responses in the brain:a short review of psychophysiological correlates and functional significance.Int.J.

Psychophysiol.24,101–112.

Beatty,W.W.,Hames,K.A.,Blanco, C.R.,Nixon,S.J.,Tivis,L.J., 1996.Visuospatial perception,construction and memory in alcoholism.

J.Stud.Alcohol57,136–143.

Begleiter,H.,Porjesz, B.,Tenner,M.,1980.Neuroradiological and neurophysiological evidence of brain deficits in chronic alcoholics.

Acta Psychiatr.Scand.,Suppl.286,3–13.

Bhattacharya,J.,Petsche,H.,Feldmann,U.,Rescher,B.,2001a.EEG gamma-band phase synchronization between posterior and frontal cortex during mental rotation in humans.Neurosci.Lett.311, 29–32.

Bhattacharya,J.,Petsche,H.,Pereda,E.,2001b.Long-range synchrony in the gamma band:role in music perception.J.Neurosci.21,6329–6337. Bhattacharya,J.,Shams,L.,Shimojo,S.,2002.Sound-induced illusory flash perception:role of gamma band responses.NeuroReport13, 1727–1730.

Bottger,D.,Herrmann,C.S.,von Cramon,D.Y.,2002.Amplitude differ-ences of evoked alpha and gamma oscillations in two different age groups.Int.J.Psychophysiol.45,245–251.

Braeutigam,S.,Bailey,A.J.,Swithenby,S.J.,2001.Phase-locked gamma band responses to semantic violation stimuli.Brain Res.Cogn.Brain Res.10,365–377.

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–26 22

Buckley,S.T.,Eckert,A.L.,Dodd,P.R.,2000.Expression and distribution of GABA(A)receptor subtypes in human alcoholic cerebral cortex.

Ann.N.Y.Acad.Sci.914,58–64.

Burgess, A.P.,Ali,L.,2002.Functional connectivity of gamma EEG activity is modulated at low frequency during conscious recollection.

Int.J.Psychophysiol.46,91–100.

Burle,B.,Bonnet,M.,2000.High-speed memory scanning:a behavioral argument for a serial oscillatory model.Brain Res.Cogn.Brain Res.9, 327–337.

Chen,Y.,Ding,M.,Kelso,J.A.,2003.Task-related power and coherence changes in neuromagnetic activity during visuomotor coordination.

Exp.Brain Res.148,105–116.

Chester,J.A.,Cunningham,C.L.,2002.GABA(A)receptor modulation of the rewarding and aversive effects of ethanol.Alcohol26,131–143. Chu,P.C.,1996.The S-transform for obtaining localized spectra.MTS J.

29,28–38.

Clark,V.P.,Fannon,S.,Lai,S.,Benson,R.,Bauer,L.,2000.Responses to rare visual target and distractor stimuli using event-related fMRI.J.

Neurophysiol.83,3133–3139.

Cohen,J.D.,Servan-Schreiber,D.,1992.Context,cortex,and dopamine:a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia.

Psychol.Rev.99,45–77.

Cohen,H.L.,Wang,W.,Porjesz,B.,Bauer,L.,Kuperman,S.,O’Connor, S.J.,Rohrbaugh,J.,Begleiter,H.,1994.Visual P300:an interlaboratory consistency study.Alcohol11,583–587.

Colling,S.B.,Stanford,I.M.,Traub,R.D.,Jefferys,J.G.,1998.Limbic gamma rhythms:I.Phase-locked oscillations in hippocampal CA1and subiculum.J.Neurophysiol.80,155–161.

Corbetta,M.,Shulman,G.L.,2002.Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain.Nat.Rev.,Neurosci.3,201–215. Davies,M.,2003.The role of GABAA receptors in mediating the effects of alcohol in the central nervous system.J.Psychiatry Neurosci.28, 263–274.

Debener,S.,Herrmann,C.S.,Kranczioch,C.,Gembris,D.,Engel,A.K., 2003.Top-down attentional processing enhances auditory evoked gamma band activity.NeuroReport14,683–686.

De Bruin, E.A.,Bijl,S.,Stam, C.J.,Bocker,K.B.,Kenemans,J.L., Verbaten,M.N.,2004.Abnormal EEG synchronisation in heavily drinking students.Clin.Neurophysiol.115,2048–2055. Desimone,R.,Duncan,J.,1995.Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.Annu.Rev.Neurosci.18,193–222.

Desmedt,J.E.,Tomberg, C.,1994.Transient phase-locking of40Hz electrical oscillations in prefrontal and parietal human cortex reflects the process of conscious somatic perception.Neurosci.Lett.168, 126–129.

Edenberg,H.J.,Dick,D.M.,Xuei,X.,Tian,H.,Almasy,L.,Bauer,L.O., Crowe,R.R.,Goate,A.,Hesselbrock,V.,Jones,K.,Kwon,J.,Li,T.K., Nurnberger,J.I.,O’Connor,S.J.,Reich,T.,Rice,J.,Schuckit,M.A., Porjesz,B.,Foroud,T.,Begleiter,H.,2004.Variations in GABRA2, encoding the alpha2subunit of the GABA(A)receptor,are associated with alcohol dependence and with brain oscillations.Am.J.Hum.

Genet.74,705–714.

Elliott,M.A.,Muller,H.J.,2000.Evidence for40-Hz oscillatory short-term visual memory revealed by human reaction-time measurements.J.Exp.

Psychol.Learn.Mem.Cogn.26,703–718.

Engel, A.K.,Fries,P.,Singer,W.,2001.Dynamic predictions:oscil-lations and synchrony in top-down processing.Nat.Rev.,Neurosci.2, 704–716.

Fell,J.,Fernandez,G.,Klaver,P.,Elger, C.E.,Fries,P.,2003.Is synchronized neuronal gamma activity relevant for selective attention?

Brain Res.Brain Res.Rev.42,265–272.

Friedman,H.R.,Goldman-Rakic,P.S.,1994.Coactivation of prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex in working memory tasks revealed by2DG functional mapping in the rhesus monkey.J.Neurosci.14, 2775–2788.

Fuster,J.M.,1989.The Prefrontal Cortex,2nd edn R Raven Press,New York.

255pp.Gabor, D.,1946.Theory of communications.J.Inst.Electr.Eng.93, 429–457.

Gansler,D.A.,Harris,G.J.,Oscar-Berman,M.,Streeter,C.,Lewis,R.F., Ahmed,I.,Achong,D.,2000.Hypoperfusion of inferior frontal brain regions in abstinent alcoholics:a pilot SPECT study.J.Stud.Alcohol 61,32–37.

Giancola,P.R.,Moss,H.B.,1998.Executive cognitive functioning in alcohol use disorders.Recent Dev.Alcohol.14,227–251.

Gilman,S.,Koeppe,R.A.,Adams,K.,Johnson-Greene,D.,Junck,L., Kluin,K.J.,Brunberg,J.,Martorello,S.,Lohman,M.,1996.Positron emission tomographic studies of cerebral benzodiazepine-receptor binding in chronic alcoholics.Ann.Neurol.40,163–171.

Green,M.F.,Mintz,J.,Salveson,D.,Nuechterlein,K.H.,Breitmeyer,B., Light,G.A.,Braff, D.L.,2003.Visual masking as a probe for abnormal gamma range activity in schizophrenia.Biol.Psychiatry53, 1113–1119.

Grice,S.J.,Spratling,M.W.,Karmiloff-Smith,A.,Halit,H.,Csibra,G.,de Haan,M.,Johnson,M.H.,2001.Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome.Neuro-Report12,2697–2700.

Grobin,A.C.,Matthews,D.B.,Devaud,L.L.,Morrow,A.L.,1998.The role of GABA(A)receptors in the acute and chronic effects of ethanol.

Psychopharmacology(Berl.)139,2–19.

Gurtubay,I.G.,Alegre,M.,Labarga,A.,Malanda,A.,Iriarte,J.,Artieda,J., 2001.Gamma band activity in an auditory oddball paradigm studied with the wavelet transform.Clin.Neurophysiol.112,1219–1228. Hada,M.,Porjesz, B.,Begleiter,H.,Polich,J.,2000.Auditory P3a assessment of male alcoholics.Biol.Psychiatry48,276–286. Haig, A.R.,De Pascalis,V.,Gordon, E.,1999.Peak gamma latency correlated with reaction time in a conventional oddball paradigm.Clin.

Neurophysiol.110,158–165.

Haig,A.R.,Gordon,E.,De Pascalis,V.,Meares,R.A.,Bahramali,H., Harris, A.,2000a.Gamma activity in schizophrenia:evidence of impaired network binding?Clin.Neurophysiol.111,1461–1468. Haig,A.R.,Gordon,E.,Wright,J.J.,Meares,R.A.,Bahramali,H.,2000b.

Synchronous cortical gamma-band activity in task-relevant cognition.

NeuroReport11,669–675.

Harper,C.G.,Kril,J.J.,Holloway,R.L.,1985.Brain shrinkage in chronic alcoholics:a pathological study.Br.Med.J.(Clin.Res.Ed.)290, 501–504.

Herrmann,C.S.,Mecklinger,A.,2000a.Gamma activity in human EEG is related to high-speed memory comparisons during object selective attention.Vis.Cogn.8,593–608.

Herrmann, C.S.,Mecklinger, A.,2000b.Magnetoencephalographic responses to illusory figures:early evoked gamma is affected by processing of stimulus features.Int.J.Psychophysiol.38,265–281. Herrmann,C.S.,Mecklinger,A.,Pfeifer,E.,1999.Gamma responses and ERPs in a visual classification task.Clin.Neurophysiol.110,636–642. Jaaskelainen,I.P.,Hirvonen,J.,Saher,M.,Pekkonen,E.,Sillanaukee,P., Naatanen,R.,Tiitinen,H.,2000.Dose-dependent suppression by ethanol of transient auditory40-Hz response.Psychopharmacology (Berl.)148,132–135.

Jensen,O.,Lisman,J.E.,1998.An oscillatory short-term memory buffer model can account for data on the Sternberg task.J.Neurosci.18, 10688–10699.

Jones,B.M.,1971.Verbal and spatial intelligence in short and long term alcoholics.J.Nerv.Ment.Dis.153,292–297.

Jones, B.,Parsons,O.A.,1972.Specific vs generalized deficits of abstracting ability in chronic alcoholics.Arch.Gen.Psychiatry26, 380–384.

Jones,K.A.,Porjesz,B.,Almasy,L.,Bierut,L.,Goate,A.,Wang,J.C., Dick,D.M.,Hinrichs,A.,Kwon,J.,Rice,J.P.,Rohrbaugh,J.,Stock,H., Wu,W.,Bauer,L.O.,Chorlian,D.B.,Crowe,R.R.,Edenberg,H.J., Foroud,T.,Hesselbrock,V.,Kuperman,S.,Nurnberger Jr.,J., O’Connor,S.J.,Schuckit,M.A.,Stimus,A.T.,Tischfield,J.A.,Reich, T.,Begleiter,H.,2004.Linkage and linkage disequilibrium of evoked EEG oscillations with CHRM2receptor gene polymorphisms:implica-

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–2623

tions for human brain dynamics and cognition.Int.J.Psychophysiol.

53,75–90.

Kaiser,J.,Lutzenberger,W.,2001.Parietal gamma-band activity during auditory spatial precueing of motor responses.NeuroReport12, 3479–3482.

Kamarajan, C.,Porjesz, B.,Jones,K.A.,Choi,K.,Chorlian, D.B., Padmanabhapillai,A.,Rangaswamy,M.,Stimus,A.T.,Begleiter,H., 2004.The role of brain oscillations as functional correlates of cognitive systems:a study of frontal inhibitory control in alcoholism.Int.J.

Psychophysiol.51,155–180.

Karakas,S.,Basar,E.,1998.Early gamma response is sensory in origin:a conclusion based on cross-comparison of results from multiple experimental paradigms.Int.J.Psychophysiol.31,13–31. Karakas,S.,Erzengin,O.U.,Basar,E.,2000.A new strategy involving multiple cognitive paradigms demonstrates that ERP components are determined by the superposition of oscillatory responses.Clin.Neuro-physiol.111,1719–1732.

Karakas,S.,Basar-Eroglu,C.,Ozesmi,C.,Kafadar,H.,Erzengin,O.U., 2001.Gamma response of the brain:a multifunctional oscillation that represents bottom-up with top-down processing.Int.J.Psychophysiol.

39,137–150.

Karakas,S.,Bekci,B.,Erzengin,O.U.,2003.Early gamma response in human neuroelectric activity is correlated with neuropsychological test scores.Neurosci.Lett.340,37–40.

Kastner,S.,Ungerleider,L.G.,2000.Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex.Annu.Rev.Neurosci.23,315–341.

Kenward,M.G.,Roger,J.H.,1997.Small sample inference for fixed effects from restricted maximum likelihood.Biometrics53,983–997. Knight,R.T.,Scabini,D.,Woods,D.L.,1989.Prefrontal cortex gating of auditory transmission in humans.Brain Res.504,338–342. Knight,R.T.,Staines,W.R.,Swick,D.,Chao,L.L.,1999.Prefrontal cortex regulates inhibition and excitation in distributed neural networks.Acta Psychol.(Amst.)101,159–178.

Krause, C.M.,Aromaki, A.,Sillanmaki,L.,Astrom,T.,Alanko,K., Salonen,E.,Peltola,O.,2002.Alcohol-induced alterations in ERD/ERS during an auditory memory task.Alcohol26,145–153.

Lachaux,J.P.,Chavez,M.,Lutz,A.,2003.A simple measure of correlation across time,frequency and space between continuous brain signals.J.

Neurosci.Methods123,175–188.

Lee,K.H.,Williams,L.M.,Breakspear,M.,Gordon,E.,2003.Synchro-nous gamma activity:a review and contribution to an integrative neuroscience model of schizophrenia.Brain Res.Brain Res.Rev.41, 57–78.

Le Van Quyen,M.,Adam,C.,Lachaux,J.P.,Martinerie,J.,Baulac,M., Renault,B.,Varela,F.J.,1997.Temporal patterns in human epileptic activity are modulated by perceptual discriminations.NeuroReport8, 1703–1710.

Lingford-Hughes,A.R.,Acton,P.D.,Gacinovic,S.,Suckling,J.,Busatto,

G.F.,Boddington,S.J.,Bullmore,E.,Woodruff,P.W.,Costa,D.C.,

Pilowsky,L.S.,Ell,P.J.,Marshall, E.J.,Kerwin,R.W.,1998.

Reduced levels of GABA-benzodiazepine receptor in alcohol depend-ency in the absence of grey matter atrophy.Br.J.Psychiatry173, 116–122.

Loring,D.W.,Sheer,D.E.,Largen,J.W.,1985.Forty Hertz EEG activity in dementia of the Alzheimer type and multi-infarct dementia.Psycho-physiology22,116–121.

Marshall,L.,Molle,M.,Bartsch,P.,1996.Event-related gamma band activity during passive and active oddball tasks.NeuroReport7, 1517–1520.

McCarthy,G.,Blamire,A.M.,Puce,A.,Nobre,A.C.,Bloch,G.,Hyder,F., Goldman-Rakic,P.,Shulman,R.G.,1994.Functional magnetic reso-nance imaging of human prefrontal cortex activation during a spatial working memory task.Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A.91,8690–8694. McCarthy,G.,Puce, A.,Constable,R.T.,Krystal,J.H.,Gore,J.C., Goldman-Rakic,P.,1996.Activation of human prefrontal cortex during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks measured by functional MRI.Cereb.Cortex6,600–611.McCarthy,G.,Luby,M.,Gore,J.,Goldman-Rakic,P.,1997.

Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI.J.Neurophysiol.77, 1630–1634.

Michael,A.,Mirza,K.A.,Mukundan,C.R.,Channabasavanna,S.M.,1993.

Interhemispheric electroencephalographic coherence as a biological marker in alcoholism.Acta Psychiatr.Scand.87,213–217.

Miller,E.K.,2000.The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control.Nat.Rev., Neurosci.1,59–65.

Miller,E.K.,Cohen,J.D.,2001.An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function.Annu.Rev.Neurosci.24,167–202.

Miltner,W.H.,Braun,C.,Arnold,M.,Witte,H.,Taub,E.,1999.Coherence of gamma-band EEG activity as a basis for associative learning.Nature 397,434–436.

Miyashita,Y.,Hayashi,T.,2000.Neural representation of visual objects:encoding and top-down activation.Curr.Opin.Neurobiol.

10,187–194.

Moselhy,H.F.,Georgiou,G.,Kahn, A.,2001.Frontal lobe changes in alcoholism:a review of the literature.Alcohol Alcohol.36, 357–368.

Muller,M.M.,Keil,A.,Kissler,J.,Gruber,T.,2001.Suppression of the auditory middle-latency response and evoked gamma-band response in

a paired-click paradigm.Exp.Brain Res.136,474–479.

Nicolas,J.M.,Catafau,A.M.,Estruch,R.,Lomena,F.J.,Salamero,M., Herranz,R.,Monforte,R.,Cardenal,C.,Urbano-Marquez,A.,1993.

Regional cerebral blood flow-SPECT in chronic alcoholism:relation to neuropsychological testing.J.Nucl.Med.34,1452–1459.

Nixon,S.J.,Bowlby, D.,1996.Evidence of alcohol-related efficiency deficits in an episodic learning task.Alcohol.,Clin.Exp.Res.20, 21–24.

O’Carroll,R.E.,Hayes,P.C.,Ebmeier,K.P.,Dougall,N.,Murray,C.,Best, J.J.,Bouchier,I.A.,Goodwin,G.M.,1991.Regional cerebral blood flow and cognitive function in patients with chronic liver disease.

Lancet337,1250–1253.

Palva,S.,Palva,J.M.,Shtyrov,Y.,Kujala,T.,Ilmoniemi,R.J.,Kaila,K., Naatanen,R.,2002.Distinct gamma-band evoked responses to speech and non-speech sounds in humans.J.Neurosci.22,RC211. Pfefferbaum,A.,Sullivan,E.V.,Mathalon,D.H.,Lim,K.O.,1997.Frontal lobe volume loss observed with magnetic resonance imaging in older chronic alcoholics.Alcohol.,Clin.Exp.Res.21,521–529. Pfefferbaum,A.,Desmond,J.E.,Galloway,C.,Menon,V.,Glover,G.H., Sullivan, E.V.,2001.Reorganization of frontal systems used by alcoholics for spatial working memory:an fMRI study.NeuroImage 14,7–20.

Porjesz,B.,Begleiter,H.,1987.Evoked brain potentials and alcoholism.In: Parsons,O.A.,Butter,N.,Nathan,P.(Eds.),Neuropsychology of Alcoholism:Implications for Diagnosis and Treatment.Guilford Press, New York,pp.45–63.vol.

Porjesz,B.,Begleiter,H.,1996.Effects of alcohol on electrophysiological activity of the brain.In:Begleiter,H.,Kissin,B.(Eds.),Alcohol and Alcoholism:The Pharmacology of Alcohol and Alcohol Dependence, vol.2.Oxford University Press,New York,pp.207–247. Porjesz,B.,Begleiter,H.,2003.Alcoholism and Human Electrophysiology.

Alcohol Res.Health27,153–160.

Porjesz,B.,Begleiter,H.,Reich,T.,Van Eerdewegh,P.,Edenberg,H.J., Foroud,T.,Goate,A.,Litke,A.,Chorlian,D.B.,Stimus,A.,Rice,J., Blangero,J.,Almasy,L.,Sorbell,J.,Bauer,L.O.,Kuperman,S., O’Connor,S.J.,Rohrbaugh,J.,1998.Amplitude of visual P3event-related potential as a phenotypic marker for a predisposition to alcoholism:preliminary results from the COGA Project.Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism.Alcohol.,Clin.Exp.Res.22, 1317–1323.

Porjesz,B.,Almasy,L.,Edenberg,H.J.,Wang,K.,Chorlian,D.B.,Foroud, T.,Goate,A.,Rice,J.P.,O’Connor,S.J.,Rohrbaugh,J.,Kuperman,S., Bauer,L.O.,Crowe,R.R.,Schuckit,M.A.,Hesselbrock,V.,Conneally, P.M.,Tischfield,J.A.,Li,T.K.,Reich,T.,Begleiter,H.,2002.Linkage disequilibrium between the beta frequency of the human EEG and a

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–26 24

GABAA receptor gene locus.Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A.99, 3729–3733.

Rangaswamy,M.,Porjesz,B.,Ardekani,B.A.,Choi,S.J.,Tanabe,J.L., Lim,K.O.,Begleiter,H.,2004.A functional MRI study of visual oddball:evidence for frontoparietal dysfunction in subjects at risk for alcoholism.NeuroImage21,329–339.

Rao,S.G.,Williams,G.V.,Goldman-Rakic,P.S.,2000.Destruction and creation of spatial tuning by disinhibition:GABA(A)blockade of prefrontal cortical neurons engaged by working memory.J.Neurosci.

20,485–494.

Ratti,M.T.,Bo,P.,Giardini,A.,Soragna,D.,2002.Chronic alcoholism and the frontal lobe:which executive functions are imparied?Acta Neurol.

Scand.105,276–281.

Rodriguez-Holguin,S.,Porjesz,B.,Chorlian,D.B.,Polich,J.,Begleiter,H., 1999.Visual P3a in Male Alcoholics and Controls.Alcohol.,Clin.Exp.

Res.23,582–591.

Salenius,S.,Salmelin,R.,Neuper,C.,Pfurtscheller,G.,Hari,R.,1996.

Human cortical40Hz rhythm is closely related to EMG rhythmicity.

Neurosci.Lett.213,75–78.

Sannita,W.G.,Conforto,S.,Lopez,L.,Narici,L.,1999.Synchronized approximately15.0–35.0Hz oscillatory response to spatially modu-lated visual patterns in man.Neuroscience89,619–623.

Sannita,W.G.,Bandini,F.,Beelke,M.,De Carli,F.,Carozzo,S.,Gesino,

D.,Mazzella,L.,Ogliastro,C.,Narici,L.,2001.Time dynamics of

stimulus-and event-related gamma band activity:contrast-VEPs and the visual P300in man.Clin.Neurophysiol.112,2241–2249. Schack,B.,Vath,N.,Petsche,H.,Geissler,H.G.,Moller,E.,2002.Phase-coupling of theta–gamma EEG rhythms during short-term memory processing.Int.J.Psychophysiol.44,143–163.

Schall,J.D.,2001.Neural basis of deciding,choosing and acting.Nat.Rev., Neurosci.2,33–42.

Schultz,W.,2000.Multiple reward signals in the brain.Nat.Rev.,Neurosci.

1,199–207.

Schurmann,M.,Basar-Eroglu,C.,Kolev,V.,Basar,E.,1995.A new metric for analyzing single-trial event-related potentials(ERPs): application to human visual P300delta response.Neurosci.Lett.

197,167–170.

Schurmann,M.,Basar-Eroglu,C.,Basar,E.,1997.Gamma responses in the EEG:elementary signals with multiple functional correlates.Neuro-Report8,1793–1796.

Senkowski,D.,Herrmann,C.S.,2002.Effects of task difficulty on evoked gamma activity and ERPs in a visual discrimination task.Clin.

Neurophysiol.113,1742–1753.

Skinner,J.E.,Yingling,C.D.,1976.Regulation of slow potential shifts in nucleus reticularis thalami by the mesencephalic reticular formation and the frontal granular cortex.Electroencephalogr.Clin.Neurophysiol.40, 288–296.

Smith,R.S.,Maes,M.,1995.The macrophage-T-lymphocyte theory of schizophrenia:additional evidence.Med.Hypotheses45,135–141. Spencer,K.M.,Nestor,P.G.,Niznikiewicz,M.A.,Salisbury,D.F.,Shenton, M.E.,McCarley,R.W.,2003.Abnormal neural synchrony in schizo-phrenia.J.Neurosci.23,7407–7411.

Stefanics,G.,Jakab,A.,Bernath,L.,Kellenyi,L.,Hernadi,I.,2004.EEG early evoked gamma-band synchronization reflects object recognition in visual oddball tasks.Brain Topogr.16,261–264.

Stockwell,R.G.,Mansinha,L.,Lowe,R.P.,1996.Localization of the complex spectrum:the S-transform.IEEE Trans.Signal Process.44, 998–1001.

Stopfer,M.,Bhagavan,S.,Smith,B.H.,Laurent,G.,1997.Impaired odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural assem-blies.Nature390,70–74.

Stuss,D.T.,Benson,D.F.,1986.The Frontal Lobes.Raven Press,New York.293pp.

Sullivan, E.V.,Mathalon, D.H.,Zipursky,R.B.,Kersteen-Tucker,Z., Knight,R.T.,Pfefferbaum,A.,1993.Factors of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test as measures of frontal-lobe function in schizophrenia and in chronic alcoholism.Psychiatry Res.46,175–199.Sullivan,E.V.,Fama,R.,Rosenbloom,M.J.,Pfefferbaum,A.,2002.A profile of neuropsychological deficits in alcoholic women.Neuro-psychology16,74–83.

Tallon-Baudry, C.,Bertrand,O.,1999.Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation.Trends Cogn.Sci.3, 151–162.

Tallon-Baudry,C.,Bertrand,O.,Delpuech,C.,Pernier,J.,1996.Stimulus specificity of phase-locked and non-phase-locked40Hz visual responses in human.J.Neurosci.16,4240–4249.

Tallon-Baudry,C.,Kreiter,A.,Bertrand,O.,1999.Sustained and transient oscillatory responses in the gamma and beta bands in a visual short-term memory task in humans.Vis.Neurosci.16,449–459.

Tarter,R.E.,1973.An analysis of cognitive deficits in chronic alcoholics.J.

Nerv.Ment.Dis.157,138–147.

Theophanis,M.R.,Queen,J.,2000.Color display of the localized spectrum.

Geophysics65,1330–1340.

Tiitinen,H.,Sinkkonen,J.,Reinikainen,K.,Alho,K.,Lavikainen,J., Naatanen,R.,1993.Selective attention enhances the auditory40-Hz transient response in humans.Nature364,59–60.

Tiitinen,H.,May,P.,Naatanen,R.,1997.The transient40-Hz response, mismatch negativity,and attentional processes in humans.Prog.Neuro-Psychopharmacol.Biol.Psychiatry21,751–771.

Traub,R.D.,Whittington,M.A.,Colling,S.B.,Buzsaki,G.,Jefferys,J.G., 1996.Analysis of gamma rhythms in the rat hippocampus in vitro and in vivo.J.Physiol.493(Pt.2),471–484.

Umeno,K.,Hori,E.,Tabuchi,E.,Takakura,H.,Miyamoto,K.,Ono,T., Nishijo,H.,2003.Gamma-band EEGs predict autonomic responses during mental arithmetic.NeuroReport14,477–480.

Wang,G.J.,V olkow,N.D.,Roque,C.T.,Cestaro,V.L.,Hitzemann,R.J., Cantos,E.L.,Levy,A.V.,Dhawan,A.P.,1993.Functional importance of ventricular enlargement and cortical atrophy in healthy subjects and alcoholics as assessed with PET.MR imaging,and neuropsychologic testing.Radiology186,59–65.

Watanabe,N.,Hirai,N.,Maehara,T.,Kawai,K.,Shimizu,H.,Miwakeichi,

F.,Uchida,S.,2002.The relationship between the visually evoked P300

event-related potential and gamma band oscillation in the human medial and basal temporal lobes.An electrocorticographic study.Neurosci.

Res.44,421–427.

Whittington,M.A.,Traub,R.D.,Jefferys,J.G.,1995.Synchronized oscillations in interneuron networks driven by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation.Nature373,612–615.

Whittington,M.A.,Stanford,I.M.,Colling,S.B.,Jefferys,J.G.,Traub, R.D.,1997.Spatiotemporal patterns of gamma frequency oscillations tetanically induced in the rat hippocampal slice.J.Physiol.502(Pt.3), 591–607.

Whittington,M.A.,Faulkner,H.J.,Doheny,H.C.,Traub,R.D.,2000a.

Neuronal fast oscillations as a target site for psychoactive drugs.

Pharmacol.Ther.86,171–190.

Whittington,M.A.,Traub,R.D.,Kopell,N.,Ermentrout,B.,Buhl,E.H., 2000b.Inhibition-based rhythms:experimental and mathematical observations on network dynamics.Int.J.Psychophysiol.38, 315–336.

Wilkinson,D.A.,Poulos,C.X.,1987.The chronic effects of alcohol on memory.A contrast between a unitary and dual system approach.

Recent Dev.Alcohol.5,5–26.

Yamaguchi,S.,Knight,R.T.,1990.Gating of somatosensory input by human prefrontal cortex.Brain Res.521,281–288.

Yordanova,J.,Kolev,V.,1996.Brain theta response predicts P300latency in children.NeuroReport8,277–280.

Yordanova,J.,Kolev,V.,Demiralp,T.,1997a.Effects of task variables on the amplitude and phase-locking of auditory gamma band responses in human.Int.J.Neurosci.92,241–258.

Yordanova,J.,Kolev,V.,Demiralp,T.,1997b.The phase-locking of auditory gamma band responses in humans is sensitive to task processing.NeuroReport8,3999–4004.

Yordanova,J.,Banaschewski,T.,Kolev,V.,Woerner,W.,Rothenberger,A., 2001.Abnormal early stages of task stimulus processing in children

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–2625

with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder-evidence from event-related gamma oscillations.Clin.Neurophysiol.112,1096–1108. Yoshiura,T.,Zhong,J.,Shibata, D.K.,Kwok,W.E.,Shrier, D.A., Numaguchi,Y.,1999.Functional MRI study of auditory and visual oddball tasks.NeuroReport10,1683–1688.Zhang,X.L.,Begleiter,H.,Porjesz,B.,Litke,A.,1997a.Electrophysio-logical evidence of memory impairment in alcoholic patients.Biol.

Psychiatry42,1157–1171.

Zhang,X.L.,Begleiter,H.,Projesz,B.,1997b.Is working memory intact in alcoholics?An ERP study.Psychiatry Res.75,75–89.

A.Padmanabhapillai et al./International Journal of Psychophysiology60(2006)15–26 26

实验一 IO地址译码器实验

计算机科学系实验报告 课程名称微型计算机技术及应用实验班级 实验名称实验一 IO地址译码器指导教师 学生姓名学生学号 一、实验目的 掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理。 二、实验设备 1.PC机一台 2.专用导线若干 3.TPC-H通用微机接口实验系统一台 4.MASM汇编及调试程序 三、实验原理和内容 实验电路如下图所示,其中74LS74为D触发器,可直接使用实验台上数字电路实验区的D触发器,74LS138为地址译码器。译码输出端Y0~Y7在实验台上“I/O地址“输出端引出,每个输出端包含8个地址,Y0:280H~287H,Y1:288H~28FH,…… 当CPU执行I/ O指令且地址在280H~2BFH范围内,译码器选中,必有一根译码线输出负脉冲。 例如:执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,2A0H OUT DX,AL(或IN AL,DX) Y4输出一个负脉冲,执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,2A8H OUT DX,AL(或IN AL,DX) Y5输出一个负脉冲。

利用这个负脉冲控制L7闪烁发光(亮、灭、亮、灭、……),时间间隔通过软件延时实现。实验的流程图如下: 四、程序代码 code segment assume cs: code

start: mov dx,2a0h out dx,al call delay ;调延时子程序 mov dx,2a8h out dx,al call delay ;调延时子程序 mov ah,1 int 16h je start ;无键按下转start mov ah,4ch int 21h delay proc near ;延时子程序 mov bx,5 lll: mov cx,0 ll: loop ll dec bx jne lll ret delay endp code ends end start 五、实验现象 试验电路中D触发器CLK端输入脉冲时,上升沿使Q段输出高电平L7发光,CD端加低电平L7灭。本试验用74LS138的Y4端口控制CLK,用Y5端口控制CD。 通过循环改变74LS138的有效输出实现二极管的闪烁。 六、心得体会 虽然实验室里很多机子不能用,但是,先看别的同学做过后,自己再动手实验,效果就比较好一点。通过这次实验,我基本了解了微机接口实验系统和TCP-USB微机接口试验系统开发环境,对I/O地址译码的内容与工作原理有了深一步的理解,切实体会到了编写程序需要的细心耐烦,另外,设计电路时也可以采用多种方法,优化结构,提高效率。

实验一 IO地址译码 实验报告

信息学院 《汇编语言与接口技术》上机实验报告 学号:104100197 姓名:王飞班级:计科10C 课程名称:汇编语言与接口技术上机内容I/O地址译码 实验性质:□综合性实验□设计性实验■验证实验 实验时间: 2012年 5 月11 日实验地点:睿智4号102室实验设备TPC—2003A微机实验箱 实验报告:(包括目的、方法、原理、结果或实验小节等)。 一、实验目的 掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理。 二、实验原理和内容 实验电路如下图所示,其中74LS74为D触发器,可直接使用实验台上数字电路实验区的D 触发器,74LS138为地址译码器。译码输出端Y0~Y7在实验台上“I/O地址“输出端引出,每 个输出端包含8个地址,Y0:280H~287H,Y1:288H~28FH,……当CPU执行I/ O指令且地 址在280H~2BFH范围内,译码器选中,必有一根译码线输出负脉冲。 利用这个负脉冲控制L7闪烁发光(亮、灭、亮、灭、……),时间间隔通过软件延时实现。 三、实验中使用的程序 stack1 segment stack 'stack' dw 32 dup(0) stack1 ends data segment baseport equ 0ec00h-280h;实际基址 port1 equ baseport+2a0h;基址+偏移地址 port2 equ baseport+2a8h;基址+偏移地址 data ends code segment

assume ss:stack1,ds:data,cs:code start: mov ax,data mov ds,ax again: mov dx, port1 in al, dx call delay ;调用延时程序 mov dx, port2 in al, dx call delay jmp again delay proc push bx push cx mov bx, 5000 wait2: mov cx,0 wait1: loop wait1 dec bx jnz wait2 pop cx pop bx ret delay endp;延时程序 mov ah, 4ch int 21h code ends end start 四、实验小结 通过本次实验,基本掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理。会写延时程序。在实验中达到了预期灯泡一亮一灭的效果。自己可以控制灯泡亮灭的速度。 任课教师评语: 教师签字:年月日注:每学期至少有一次设计性实验。每学期结束请任课教师按时按量统一交到教学秘书处。

微机原理( IO地址译码) 实验报告

本科学生实验报告 学号114090433 姓名李艳茹 学院物电学院专业、班级11应电 实验课程名称微机原理与接口技术实验 教师及职称罗永道(教授) 开课学期2013 至2014 学年上学期填报时间2013 年9 月10 日 云南师范大学教务处编印

一、实验预习 实验序号01 实验名称 I/O地址译码 实验时间2011年9月10日实验室微机原理与接口技术实验室 1.实验目的 掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理。 2.实验原理、实验流程或装置示意图 实验电路如图1-1所示,其中74LS74为D触发器,可直接使用实验台上数字电路实验区的D触发器,74LS138为地址译码器。译码输出端Y0~Y7在实验台上“I/O地址“输出端引出,每个输出端包含8个地址,Y0:280H~287H,Y1:288H~28FH,……当CPU执行I/O指令且地址在280H~2BFH范围内,译码器选中,必有一根译码线输出负脉冲。 例如:执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,2A0H OUT DX,AL(或IN AL,DX) Y4输出一个负脉冲,执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,2A8H OUT DX,AL(或IN AL,DX) Y5输出一个负脉冲。 图1-1 利用这个负脉冲控制L7闪烁发光(亮、灭、亮、灭、……),时间间隔通过软件延时实现。

3.实验参考程序为: ;*******************************; ;* I/O地址译码 *; ;*******************************; outport1 equ2a0h outport2 equ2a8h code segment assume cs:code start: mov dx,outport1 out dx,al call delay ;调延时子程序 mov dx,outport2 out dx,al call delay ;调延时子程序 mov ah,1 int16h je start mov ah,4ch int21h delay proc near;延时子程序 mov bx,200 lll: mov cx,0 ll: loop ll dec bx jne lll ret delay endp code ends end start 4.实验设备及材料 电脑一台、微机原理与接口技术实验箱一个、导线若干、电源相应的模拟软件等。 5.实验方法步骤及注意事项 实验步骤: 1)学习理解实验的目的,明确做该实验的意义; 2)在检查好实验仪器后,按照实验原理图一连接好电路; 3)打开电源,在电脑上检测硬件是否连接;

IO地址译码

物理与电子科学系实验报告 课程名称 微机原理与接口技术 实验班级 B13电子班 实验名称 I/O 地址译码 学生姓名 学生学号 一、实验目的 掌握I/O 地址译码电路的工作原理。 二、实验内容 实验电路如图1-1所示,图中线路两端有节点的信号线需要用户用实验导线连接起来,其中74LS74为D 触发器,可直接使用实验台上部系统板上的D 触发器。74LS138为地址译码器。译码输出端Y0-Y7在实验台中间系统板上引出,每个输出端包含8个地址,即: Y0:280H ~287H ; Y4:2A0H ~2A7H; Y1:288H ~28FH; Y 5:2A8H ~2AFH; Y2:290H ~297H; Y 6:2B0H ~2B7H; Y3:298H ~29FH; Y 7:2B8H ~2BFH; G16 G2A 45 G2B Y4Y5Y6Y7 111097A 1B 23C Y0Y1Y2Y315141312 Y4Y5Y6Y7 Y0Y1Y2 Y3CLK 1Q 6 D 2PRE 4 Q 5CLK 3+5V GND VCC A3 A4A5 8 L0 SN7474 SN74LS138 U14A U18U17SN74LS30 U16D SN74LS00 U16C SN74LS00 U16B SN74LS00 U16A SN74LS00 11 8 6 3 12345678 1213 9 10 4 512 A6A7 A8A9 AEN IOR IOW 图1-1 I/O 地址译码电路图 例如:执行下面两条程序 MOV DX, 2AOH OUT DX, AL Y4输出一个负脉冲,执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,2A8H OUT DX,AL Y5输出一个负脉冲。 计算出的地址=查找出的PCI 卡的基址+偏移量

io地址译码

一、实验目的 掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理。 二、实验原理和内容 实验电路如图1所示,其中74LS74为D触发器,可直接使用实验台上数字电路实验区的D 触发器,74LS138为地址译码器。译码输出端Y0~Y7在实验台上I/O地址“输出端引出,每个输出端包含8个地址,Y0:280H~287H,Y1:288H~28FH,……当CPU执行I/ O指令且地址在280H~2BFH范围内,译码器选中,必有一根译码线输出负脉冲。 注意:命令中的端口地址 D820、D82A 是根据PCI卡的基址再加上偏移量计算出来的,不同的微机器PCI卡的基址可能不同,需要事先查找出来。 计算公式如下:计算出的地址查找出的PCI卡的基址+偏移量;(其中:偏移量 =2A0H - 280H或 2A8H –A80H) 图1 利用这个负脉冲控制L7闪烁发光(亮、灭、亮、灭、……),时间间隔通过软件延时实现。 三、编程提示 1、实验电路中D触发器CLK端输入脉冲时,上升沿使Q端输出高电平L7发光,CD端加低电平L7灭。 2、由于TPC卡使用PCI总线,所以分配的IO地址每台微机可能都不同,编程时需要了解当前的微机使用那段IO地址并进行设置,获取方法请参看汇编程序使用方法的介绍。(也可使用自动获取资源分配的程序取得中断号)。 四、实验代码 CODE SEGMENT ASSUME CS:CODE START: LOOP1: MOV CX,0FFFFH LP1:

MOV DX,2AOH IN AL,DX LOOP LP1 MOV CX,0FFFFH LP2: NOP LOOP LP2 MOV CX,0FFFFH LP3: MOV DX,2A8H IN AL,DX LOOP LP3 MOV CX,0FFFFH LP4: NOP LOOP LP4 MOV AH,0BH INT 21H CMP AL,0 JZ LOOP1 MOV AH,4CH INT 21H CODE ENDS END START 五、实验总结 通过实验,了解和掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理,熟悉汇编代码的编写。实验中,连接电路,利用代码控制实验电路,深对课本理论的理解。

IO地址译码

实验一I/O地址译码 一.实验内容: 1.实验电路如图所示。 74LS74为D触发器,可直接使用实验台上数字电路实验区的D触发器,74LS138为地址译码器。 译码输出端Y0~Y7在实验台上“I/O地址“输出端引出,每个输出端包含8个地址,Y0:280H~287H,Y1:288H~28FH……当CPU执行I/O指令且地址在280H~2BFH范围内,译码器选中,必有一根译码线输出负脉冲。 2.接线: Y4/IO地址接CLK/D触发器 Y5/IO地址接CD/D触发器 D/D触发器接SD/D角发器接+5V Q/D触发器接逻辑笔 二.实验目的:掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理 三.实验仪器: TPC-ZK实验系统中的IO地址、D触发器、逻辑笔模块 USB核心板 HQFC集成开发环境 四.实验总体思路: 实验电路中D触发器CLK端输入脉冲时,上升沿使Q端输出高电平L7发光,

CD端加低电平L7灭。例如:执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,2A0H OUT DX,AL(或IN AL,DX) Y4输出一个负脉冲,执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,2A8H OUT DX,AL(或IN AL,DX) Y5输出一个负脉冲。 编写实验程序: CODES SEGMENT ASSUME CS:CODES START PROC NEAR MOV DX,2A0H OUT DX,AL CALL DELAY MOV DX,2A8H OUT DX,AL CALL DELAY JE START DELAY PROC NEAR MOV BX,200 LL:MOV CX,0 L:LOOP L DEC BX JNE LL RET DELAY ENDP 五.实验步骤: 1、将实验板安好,连接至电脑。 2、开启电源,运行HQPC,查找接口 3、点击USB接口存在,进入USB微机接口开发环境 4、输入程序,编译源程序 5、无误后调试、运行程序 六.实验结果: 程序正确运行后,灯L7闪烁发光(亮、灭、亮、灭……) 七.实验心得:(包含所遇到的问题及解决方法) 备注:所有文字一律采用小四,宋体。

I O地址译码(微机实验报告)

I/O地址译码 一、实验目的 掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理。 二、实验原理和内容 实验电路如图1所示,其中74LS74为D触发器,可直接使用实验台上数字电路实验区的D 触发器,74LS138为地址译码器。译码输出端Y0~Y7在实验台上I/O地址“输出端引出,每个输出端包含8个地址,Y0:280H~287H,Y1:288H~28FH,……当CPU执行I/ O指令且地址在280H~2BFH范围内,译码器选中,必有一根译码线输出负脉冲。 注意:命令中的端口地址D820、D82A 是根据PCI卡的基址再加上偏移量计算出来的,不同的微机器PCI卡的基址可能不同,需要事先查找出来。 计算公式如下:计算出的地址查找出的PCI卡的基址+偏移量;(其中:偏移量=2A0H - 280H或2A8H –A80H) 图1 利用这个负脉冲控制L7闪烁发光(亮、灭、亮、灭、……),时间间隔通

过软件延时实现。 三、编程提示 1、实验电路中D触发器CLK端输入脉冲时,上升沿使Q端输出高电平L7发光,CD端加低电平L7灭。 2、由于TPC卡使用PCI总线,所以分配的IO地址每台微机可能都不同,编程时需要了解当前的微机使用那段IO地址并进行设置,获取方法请参看汇编程序使用方法的介绍。(也可使用自动获取资源分配的程序取得中断号)。 四、实验代码 CODE SEGMENT ASSUME CS:CODE START: LOOP1: MOV CX,0FFFFH LP1: MOV DX,2AOH IN AL,DX LOOP LP1 MOV CX,0FFFFH LP2: NOP LOOP LP2 MOV CX,0FFFFH

实验五 地址译码电路与IO接口

电工电子实验中心 实验报告 课程名称:计算机硬件技术基础 实验名称:地址译码电路与I/O接口姓名:学号: 评定成绩:审阅教师: 实验时间:2017.05.16 南京航空航天大学

1) 学习 3-8 译码器在接口电路中的应用。 2) 掌握地址译码电路的一般设计方法。 3) 理解输入输出接口的基本原理。 二、实验任务 用 74LS138 译码器设计地址译码电路,其 Y7 作为基本输入/输出单元的片选信号。参考实验电路如图 3-1-1 所示,功能及流程如下: 1) 读入 74LS245 输入的八位数据,在 74LS574 上输出,用八位 LED 显示开关状态。 2) 当有键按下,且读入的数字量为 1, 则八位 LED 从右向左依次循点 亮,否则重读数字量。 3) 再有键按下,且读入的数字量为 2, 则八位 LED 交替亮,否则重读 数字量。 4) 再有键按下返回。

图3-1-1 地址译码设计实验电原理框图 四、实验代码 IOY0 EQU 3000H ;片选IOY0对应的端口始地址Y7 EQU IOY0+0E0H ;译码电路输出Y7对应的端口地址DATA SEGMENT NUM DB 01H DATA ENDS STACK1 SEGMENT STACK DW 256 DUP(?) STACK1 ENDS

CODE SEGMENT ASSUME CS:CODE,DS:DATA START: MOV DX, Y7 ;读入开关量 IN AL, DX OUT DX, AL MOV AH, 1 ;判断是否有按键按下 INT 16H JZ START ;无按键则跳回继续循环,有则退出 L1: MOV DX,Y7 IN AL,DX ;读入开关量, 判断是否为1 CMP AL,1 JNE L1 L2: MOV DX,Y7 MOV AL,NUM OUT DX,AL ;八位LED从右向左依次循点亮 ROL AL,1 MOV NUM,AL CALL DELAY MOV DL,0FFH MOV AH, 6 ;判断是否有按键按下 INT 21H JZ L2 ;无按键则跳回继续循环,有则退出L3: MOV DX,Y7 IN AL,DX ; 读入开关量, 判断是否为2 CMP AL,2 JNE L3 MOV NUM,55H L4: MOV DX,Y7 MOV AL,NUM OUT DX,AL ;八位LED交替亮 NOT AL MOV NUM,AL CALL DELAY MOV DL,0FFH MOV AH, 6 ;判断是否有按键按下 INT 21H JZ L4 ;无按键则跳回继续循环,有则退出 MOV AX, 4C00H ;结束程序退出

微机原理课程IO地址译码设计报告

郑州科技学院 微机原理与接口技术 课程设计任务书 专业计算机科学与技术班级计科一班学号201215017 姓名夏飞 一、设计题目I/O地址译码 二、设计任务与要求 1、掌握I/O地址译码电路的工作原理。 2、实现走马灯产生8种彩灯(8位LED)的走马灯花样。 3、通过走马灯的设计与制作,深入了解与掌握利用可编程 8255A。 三、参考文献(不少于5个) [1] 《微机原理与接口技术》,梁建武,中国水利水电出版社,2010; [2] 《微机原理与接口技术》,雷丽文,北京:电子工业出版社1997; [3] 《微机原理及应用》,周明德,北京:清华大学出版社,1998; [4] 《微机原理与接口技术》,倪继烈,电子科技大学出版社,2004; [5] 雷丽文《微机原理与接口技术》[M] 电子工业出版社,1997.2 四、设计时间 2015 年12 月5 日至2015 年1 月11 日 指导教师签名: 2015年 1 月 5日

郑州科技学院 《微机原理与接口技术》课程设计 题目I/O地址译码 学生姓名 院(系)

目录 1 引言 (1) 2 方案讨论 (3) 2.1 方案1 (3) 2.2 方案2 (4) 2.3 方案3 (5) 2.4 个人设计方案 (5) 3 设计原理及功能 (7) 3.1 设计原理应用芯片8255A介绍 (7) 3.2 硬件电路设计 (7) 3.2.1 硬件连线 (8) 3.3软件电路设计 (10) 4 测试与结果测试 (11) 4.1 硬件检测 (11) 4.2 调试运行 (11) 4.3 实验现象与说明 (11) 4.4 结果与分析 (11) 5 总结 (12) 参考文献 (14) 附录1 (15) 附录2 (16)

IO地址译码实验

I/O地址译码实验 一、实验目的 掌握I/O 地址译码电路的工作原理。 二、实验内容说明 实验电路如图所示,其中74LS74 为D 触发器,可直接使用实验台上数字电路实验区的D 触发器,74LS138 为地址译码器。译码输出端Y0~Y7 在实验台上“I/O 地址”输出端引出,每个输出端包含8 个地址,Y0:0100H,Y1:0200H,……当CPU 执行I/ O 指令且地址在(PC基地址+0000H~007FH)范围内,译码器选中,必有一根译码线输出负脉冲。 例如:执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,( PC基地址+0000H ) OUT DX,AL( 或 IN AL,DX ) Y0 输出一个负脉冲,执行下面两条指令 MOV DX,( PC基地址+0030H ) OUT DX,AL( 或 IN AL,DX ) Y3 输出一个负脉冲。 利用这个负脉冲控制L0 闪烁发光(亮、灭、亮、灭、……),时间间隔通过软件延时实现。 三、实验步骤 1、实验连线:参见(实验原理图) 译码电路的74LS138 0000H →逻辑电路区74LS74(CLK) 译码电路的74LS138 0030H →逻辑电路区74LS74(CLR) 逻辑电路区74LS74(D ,SET)→ VCC 逻辑电路区74LS74(Q)→逻辑电平区L0 2、参考程序:D74LS138.ASM在QTH组合窗口下装入、编译、连接参考实验程序;并运行该程序。 3、通过修改参考程序中DELAY_SET的软件延时参数,观察发光二极管的变化。 四、编程提示 实验电路中D触发器CLK 端输入脉冲时,上升沿使Q 端输出高电平LED发光,CLR端加低电平LED 灭。 编程 ;******************************************************** ; /* 74LS138 I/O 译码实验*/ * ;******************************************************** ; ; 偏移地址: 0000H 用户使用片选1 ; 偏移地址: 0010H 用户使用片选2 ; 偏移地址: 0020H 用户使用片选3 ; 偏移地址: 0030H 用户使用片选4 ; 偏移地址: 0040H DA_0832 片选 ; 偏移地址: 0050H AD_0809 片选 ; 偏移地址: 0060H 8253 片选 ; 偏移地址: 0070H 8255 片选

相关文档
最新文档