2006-Dick-Endophenotypes successfully lead to gene identification

2006-Dick-Endophenotypes successfully lead to gene identification
2006-Dick-Endophenotypes successfully lead to gene identification

Endophenotypes Successfully Lead to Gene Identi?cation:Results from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism

Danielle M.Dick,1,8Kevin Jones,2Nancy Saccone,1Anthony Hinrichs,1Jen C.Wang,1Alison Goate,1Laura Bierut,1Laura Almasy,3Marc Schuckit,4Victor Hesselbrock,5Jay Tisch?eld,6Tatiana Foroud,7Howard Edenberg,7Bernice Porjesz,2and Henri Begleiter 2

Received 27Jan.2005—Final 15July 2005

The use of endophenotypes has been proposed as a strategy to aid gene identi?cation e?orts for complex phenotypes [Gottesman,I.I.,and Shields J.(1972).Schizophrenia and Genetics:A Twin Study Vantage Point .London:Academic].As part of the Collaborative Study of the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA)project,we have analyzed electrophysiological endopheno-types,in addition to clinical diagnoses,as part of our effort to identify genes involved in the predisposition to alcohol dependence.In this paper we summarize published results from linkage and association analyses of two chromosomal regions in which the use of endophenotypes has successfully led to the identi?cation of genes associated with alcohol dependence [GABRA2(Edenberg et al .,(2004).Am.J.Hum.Genet .74:705à714)and CHRM2(Wang et al .,(2004).Hum.Mol.Genet .13:1903à1911)].Our experience in the COGA project has been that the analysis of endophenotypes provides several advantages over diagnostic phenotypes,including the strength and localization of the linkage signal.Our results provide an illustration of the successful use of endophenotypes to identify genes involved in the predisposition to a complex psychiatric phenotype,a strategy originally proposed by Gottesman and Shields in 1972.

KEY WORDS:Alcohol dependence;association;EEG;endophenotype;genetics;linkage.

INTRODUCTION

Identifying genes involved in complex human behaviors and clinical disorders has proven dif?cult.Early enthusiasm (Egeland et al .,1987;Gershon

et al .,1988)was quickly curtailed (Kelsoe et al .,1989),as it became apparent that the strategy suc-cessfully employed to identify genes for many single-gene disorders would not be nearly as useful for identifying most of the genetic variation contributing to complex phenotypes.Many strategies have been adopted in gene identi?cation e?orts to deal with the complexities introduced by studying disorders that are believed to have multifactorial,polygenic origins,rather than simple Mendelian patterns of inheritance.One strategy that has been proposed is the use of endophenotypes.

Psychiatric diagnoses were formulated for the purpose of clinical classi?cation and communication between care providers.Diagnoses are based on observable symptoms and there is considerable 1Washington University School of Medicine,St.Louis,MO USA.2State University of New York,New York,NY USA.

3

Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research,San Antonio,TX USA.

4University of California,San Diego,CA USA.

5University of Connecticut Health Center,Farmington,CT USA.6Rutgers University,New Brunswick,NJ USA.

7Indiana University School of Medicine,Indianapolis,IN USA.8

To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of Psychiatry,Washington University in St.Louis,Box 8134,660South Euclid Ave.,St.Louis,MO 63110,USA.Tel.:+1-314-286-2297;Fax:+1-314-286-2213;e-mail:dickd@https://www.360docs.net/doc/a52108182.html,

112

0001-8244/06/0100-0112/0ó2006Springer Science +Business Media,Inc.

Behavior Genetics,Vol.36,No.1,January 2006(ó2006)DOI:10.1007/s10519-005-9001-3

heterogeneity within any given diagnosis.Because there is likely a complex cascade of events between the genetic underpinnings of a disorder and the eventual manifestation of symptoms,it is not clear that clinical diagnoses are the best phenotype for use in genetic analyses.A more optimal phenotype for genetic analyses might be an intermediary measure of neuropsychiatric functioning that is involved in the pathway between genotype and the outcome of interest.These intermediary phenotypes have been termed endophenotypes.The concept of endophe-notypes was?rst applied to psychiatric disorders by Gottesman and Shields(Gottesman and Shields, 1972),and a recent review provides an excellent overview of the rationale for using endophenotypes in gene identi?cation e?orts in psychiatry(Gottesman and Gould,2003).Several criteria have been delin-eated for a biological marker to represent an appro-priate endophenotype for genetic analyses (Gottesman and Gould,2003).The marker should be associated with the illness.In addition,it should be found in higher rates in the una?ected relatives of a?ected individuals than in the general population. The marker should be heritable,and there should be a genetic correlation between the trait and disorder, indicating that shared genes are contributing to the observed relationship(de Geus and Boomsma,2001).

COGA is a multidisciplinary collaborative pro-ject with the goal of identifying genes involved in alcohol dependence and related phenotypes,such as quantitative indices of alcohol use and other psychi-atric disorders,such as depression,that are com-monly comorbid with alcohol dependence.Although alcohol dependence clearly has a genetic component, with heritabilities in the range of50à60%for both men and women(Heath,1995;McGue,1999),it embodies many of the complexities inherent to gene identi?cation e?orts for most psychiatric disorders. Many genes are thought to be involved,each one likely only contributing a small e?ect.The environ-ment clearly plays an important role in the develop-ment of drinking patterns and dependence symptoms (Rose et al.,2001b;Rose et al.,2003).There is evi-dence of gene-environment interaction associated with alcohol dependence(Dick et al.,2001;Heath et al.,1989;Koopmans et al.,1999;Rose et al., 2001a).Finally,there is substantial heterogeneity among alcohol dependent individuals(Cloninger, 1987;Finn et al.,1997).

One strategy that the COGA project has employed to attempt to deal with these complexities is the use of electrophysiological endophenotypes as a complement to clinical diagnoses for use in genetic analyses.There is a substantial body of literature suggesting that electrophysiological measures repre-sent relevant endophenotypes for alcohol depen-dence.It has been proposed that the genetic predisposition to alcohol dependence may involve central nervous system(CNS)disinhibition/hyperex-citability(Begleiter and Porjesz,1999),and electro-physiological abnormalities may re?ect this CNS disinhibition.Abnormalities in alcoholics and their family members are found both in the human elec-troencephalogram(EEG),as well as evoked EEG rhythms,or event-related potentials(ERPs),such as the P300response.The electrophysiological end-ophenotypes used in linkage analyses described in this paper focus on the beta frequency band of the human EEG,and the delta and theta frequency band evoked oscillations,which are the primary constitu-ents of the P300evoked component elicited during cognitive processing of stimuli(Basar et al.,1999; Yordanova and Kolev,1996).Several lines of evi-dence suggest that these traits may represent useful endophenotypes indexing familial risk for alcohol dependence.The beta frequency band of EEG is highly heritable,with heritability estimated at86% (van Beijsterveldt et al.,1996).Thus,this endophe-notype is more highly heritable than alcohol depen-dence diagnoses themselves(McGue,1999).In the COGA sample,increased beta power in all three bands of resting EEG has been observed in alcohol dependent individuals,as compared to controls (Rangaswamy et al.,2002,2003).An increase in beta power has also been observed in the o?spring of male alcoholics,further suggesting this may be a marker of an inherited predisposition to alcohol dependence (Rangaswamy et al.,2004b).Several studies have demonstrated that a reduced P300amplitude is associated with the risk for alcoholism(e.g.,see (Begleiter et al.,1984;Carlson et al.,2004;Reese and Polich,2003)).Recent evidence indicates reduced theta and delta oscillations in alcohol dependent individuals and individuals at risk(For reviews,see (Porjesz and Begleiter,2003;Porjesz et al.,2004; Porjesz et al.,in press)).Differences in P300corre-spond to activation differences in the bilateral infe-rior parietal lobule and the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus on functional magnetic resonance imaging tests (fMRI),signifying that a dysfunctional frontoparietal circuit may be responsible for the reduced P300found in subjects at high risk for alcoholism(Rangaswamy et al.,2004a).Taken together,these results suggest that electrophysiological endophenotypes may

113

COGA Linkage

represent biological markers of the genetic predispo-sition to alcohol dependence and may have utility in genetic analyses.Another useful quality is that these endophenotypes are quantitative traits,potentially allowing for more powerful tests of linkage.

The COGA analytic strategy has been to use both electrophysiological endophenotypes(Almasy et al.,2001;Begleiter et al.,1998;Ghosh et al.,2003) and clinical diagnostic phenotypes(Foroud et al., 2000;Reich et al.,1998),as well as novel alcohol-related phenotypes(e.g.,maximum number of drinks in a24hour period(Saccone et al.,2000),alcohol factor scores(Dick et al.,2002),and an alcohol symptom severity phenotype(Foroud et al.,1998)) in genetic analyses.The study design employed by COGA has been to ascertain large families,densely a?ected with alcohol dependence;to conduct non-parametric linkage analyses to identify chromosomal regions likely to contain genes predisposing to alcohol dependence and related phenotypes;and to conduct family-based association analyses on can-didate genes within those regions of interest to identify the speci?c genes involved.We have chosen initially to focus on regions in which the electro-physiological endophenotypes and clinical pheno-types show(loosely)converging evidence of linkage. This strategy has successfully led to the identi?cation of several genes that show signi?cant association with alcohol dependence and related psychiatric phenotypes.An overview of the results from two of the most promising chromosomal regions that we have investigated to date,on chromosomes4and7, is presented here.

METHODS

Sample

Families were identi?ed through probands in inpatient and outpatient alcohol dependence treat-ment centers at six sites across the United States: Indiana University,State University of New York Health Science Center,University of Connecticut, University of Iowa,University of California/San Diego,and Washington University,St.Louis.The institutional review boards of all participating insti-tutions approved the study.Probands were invited to participate if they had a suf?ciently large family (usually sibships>3with parents available)with two or more members in any of the COGA catchment areas(Reich,1996).A total of1227families of alcohol dependent probands were recruited for the ?rst stage of the study.All individuals were admin-istered the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism(SSAGA)interview,which is a polydiagnostic instrument that assesses most major psychiatric disorders(Bucholz et al.,1994;Hessel-brock et al.,1999).Families that had at least two a?ected?rst degree relatives in addition to the pro-band(excluding probands who were the o?spring of two a?ected parents)were invited to participate in the more intensive stage of the study.In these families,all ?rst degree relatives of a?ected individuals and con-necting family members were assessed,along with their mates,if the union had produced o?spring. Second and third degree relatives in the families were assessed when they were considered to be informative for the genetic linkage studies.A total of987adult individuals from105extended families were included in the initial genotyped data set(Reich et al.,1998).

A replication sample was ascertained and genotyped following identical procedures;it consisted of1295 individuals from157extended families(Foroud et al.,2000).Thus,a total of2282individuals from 262multiplex alcoholic families are available for ge-netic analyses.An average of9individuals were genotyped per family.Of the2282genotyped indi-viduals,895met criteria for the COGA de?nition of alcoholism used for recruitment(DSMIIIR a?ection plus Feighner de?nite alcoholism);these individuals created491a?ected sibling pairs(n-1)(Foroud et al., 2000).The complete manual detailing COGA ascer-tainment is available on-line at https://www.360docs.net/doc/a52108182.html,/ niaaa/.

All individuals who participated in the intensive stage of the study underwent a more extensive pro-tocol,in addition to the SSAGA interview.This stage involved obtaining blood for genetic analyses,and an electrophysiological protocol,including EEG and a battery of auditory and visual evoked potentials. EEG was recorded using the19channel montage,as speci?ed according to the10à20international sys-tem.EEG data were collected in the awake,eyes-closed condition at a sampling rate of256Hz for 4.25minutes(see(Porjesz et al.,2002)for additional details on the procedure).EROs were measured in response to a visual oddball paradigm in which three types of visual stimuli were presented:target,non-target,and novel.Stimulus duration was60ms and the interstimulus interval was 1.6s.Subjects were requested to respond to the target stimulus by pressing a button with the left or right index?nger (complete details on the procedure provided in(Jones et al.,2004)).

114Dick et al.

Genotyping

Genotyping for the COGA project is carried out in laboratories at Indiana University and Washington University,St.Louis,as described previously(Reich et al.,1998).Standard methods for pedigree checking and generating recombination based marker maps have been employed(Foroud et al.,2000).The initial genome scan of COGA pedigrees included292 markers with an average intermarker distance of 13.8cM(Reich et al.,1998).Regions with gaps >20cM were typed more extensively,as were re-gions with suggestive linkage in the initial genomic scan.In addition,the marker maps have been rebuilt several times as physical position information became available from NCBI.Thus,the marker maps and positions di?er slightly between many of the analyses reviewed here from papers published at di?erent points in this on-going study.

For association analyses,SNPs were chosen across each candidate gene from public databases;we did not restrict ourselves to coding regions or exons, because allele frequencies for such SNPs are often low.In addition,we hypothesized that many SNPs involved in these complex phenotypes would be reg-ulatory.We aimed for even spacing and full coverage of each of the genes being tested.Locations were in most cases determined from the annotations in the NCBI human genome assembly.SNP genotyping has been conducted using a Pyrosequencing method or using a modi?ed single nucleotide extension reaction, with allele detection by mass spectrometry(Seque-nom MassArray system;Sequenom,San Diego,CA). Statistical Analyses

In general,nonparametric allele-sharing linkage methods for affected sibling pairs and extended pedigrees have been used in analyses of alcohol dependence in the COGA sample.The program AS-PEX(Hinds and Risch,1999)has been used exten-sively to conduct analyses on dichotomous traits (e.g.,a?ection status)using a?ected sibling pairs. Many semi-quantitative traits have been analyzed using the Haseman-Elston routine implemented in MAPMAKER/SIBS,as this method only assumes normality of the residuals of the quantitative trait, rather than normality of the trait itself.This has been a particular concern in many analyses of phenotypes in COGA,where the trait distribution has been skewed(e.g.,symptom counts).Variance components methods of analysis,largely implemented in the package SOLAR,developed by a COGA co-investi-gator(Almasy and Blangero,1998),have been em-ployed to analyze most of the quantitative endophenotypes.Analyses have been conducted using the t-distribution option in SOLAR,since it is less susceptible to distributional violations caused by slight kurtosis observed in many of the electrophysi-ological endophenotypes.In general,ascertainment correction has not been applied in the analyses re-ported here.In early analyses of the COGA electro-physiological traits,including an ascertainment correction had relatively little impact on the analyses (see Almasy et al.,1999);accordingly,most sub-sequent analyses have not employed ascertainment correction.

Some additional details about methods used in analyses are included in the presentation of individual studies,and we refer the reader to the original pub-lications for full details.For a nice overview of dif-ferent linkage methods appropriate for analyzing quantitative traits under different conditions see (Ferreira,2004).

RESULTS

Chromosome4

Linkage analyses of alcohol dependence diagno-ses did not detect signi?cant evidence of linkage on chromosome4in either the wave1(Reich et al.,1998) or wave2(Foroud et al.,2000)COGA samples using nonparametric methods for a?ected relative pairs. However,using related phenotypes and varying ana-lytic methods,this chromosome repeatedly emerged with interesting linkage?ndings.Figure1shows results of linkage analyses on chromosome4for the phenotype‘‘maximum number of drinks in a24hour period,’’after log-transformation and adjustment for gender(referred to as‘‘max drinks’’throughout the remainder of the paper)(Saccone et al.,2000).This variable shows a heritability of approximately50% (A.Heath,unpublished data),and was recorded as part of the SSAGA interview with the question‘‘What is the largest number of drinks you have ever had in a 24hour period?’’.Responses were log-transformed to minimize the impact of extremely high reports that were likely in?ated.Genome-wide linkage analyses were performed using MAPMAKER/SIBS multi-point Haseman-Elston(all pairs unweighted)for the wave1,wave2,and combined COGA samples.The most consistent,strongest evidence of linkage to this phenotype emerged on chromosome4,with a maxi-

115

COGA Linkage

mum lod score of 3.5in the combined sample at 121cM near the marker D4S2407.Interestingly,analyses of the wave 1data had shown evidence of increased allele-sharing among concordantly unaf-fected sibling pairs,and decreased allele-sharing among discordant sibling pairs,to a nearby region of chromosome 4,with a maximum multipoint lod score of 2.50(Reich et al .,1998).

Figure 2shows the results of variance compo-nents linkage analyses performed using the program SOLAR (Almasy and Blangero,1998)for alcoholism diagnoses using DSM-IV,ICD-10,and the COGA de?nition of alcoholism,which consisted of DSM-IIIR diagnoses plus Feighner criteria for alcoholism at the ‘‘de?nite’’level (Williams et al .,1999).Age and sex were used as covariates in all analyses.These analyses were performed on the Wave 1sample of 105pedigrees.The maximum lod score was 2.8,ob-tained with DSM-IV diagnoses of alcohol depen-dence at $100cM near the marker D4S1628.This is the same region of chromosome 4that also showed linkage to max drinks.A joint multipoint linkage analysis of multivariate discrete and continuous traits was subsequently applied in a bivariate analysis of qualitative alcoholism diagnoses and quantitative event-related potentials.For these analyses,DSM-IV alcohol dependence diagnoses were analyzed jointly with the amplitude of the P300component of the Cz event-related potential (Williams et al .,1999).The linkage peak signi?cantly increased,with a maximum lod score of 4.75.

The strongest evidence of linkage on chromo-some 4was found to the beta frequency band of the human EEG (Porjesz et al .,2002).The sample in-cluded in the linkage analysis was drawn from 250COGA families and consisted of 1553individuals between the ages of 7and 70.The EEG data for the beta 1(12.5à16.0Hz),beta 2(16.5à20.0Hz)and beta 3(20.5à28.0Hz)bands were analyzed using variance components linkage analyses,carried out in SOLAR (Almasy and Blangero,1998).The strongest evidence of linkage was found on chromosome 4p.The peak lod score for all bands occurred at the marker GABRB1,with a maximum lod score of 5.01obtained for the Beta 2phenotype (Fig.3).A com-bined linkage/linkage disequilibrium analysis was subsequently carried out in SOLAR to test for asso-ciation between the beta 2EEG phenotype and the GABRB1microsatellite https://www.360docs.net/doc/a52108182.html,ing this method,the lod score increased to 6.53,and signi?cant evi-dence of linkage disequilibrium was found between beta 2and the GABRB1marker (p =0.004),sug-gesting that a genetic variant in?uencing the beta 2phenotype was in or near the GABRB1

microsatellite

Multipoint LOD scores for chromosome 4for the phenotype ‘‘maximum number of drinks in a 24hour period’’(M),log-transformed

and corrected for gender (Saccone et al .,2000).

116Dick et al.

marker (Porjesz et al .,2002).Signi?cant linkage with the beta 2EEG trait was also detected on chromo-some 4using a novel nonparametric regression pro-cedure (Ghosh et al .,2003):two distinct peaks emerged,with the strongest region of linkage at the GABRB1marker,and a slightly smaller peak near the ADH3marker (the same region where the peak is located for the max drinks and dependence diagnosis analyses discussed above).Family-based association analyses of the microsatellite marker in GABRB1also provided modest evidence of association with alcohol dependence in the COGA pedigrees (Song et al .,2003).Analyses of alcohol dependence in an independent sample of Southwestern American Indians provided evidence of linkage to a marker very near the GABRB1gene (Long et al .,1998).

GABRB1is located within a tight cluster of four GABA A receptor genes on chromosome 4p:GAB-RG1,GABRA2,GABRA4,and GABRB1.Evidence from animal,human,and in vitro cell models suggests that c aminobutyric acid (GABA),the major inhibi-tory neurotransmitter in the human central nervous system,is involved in many of the neurochemical pathways affecting alcohol use and related disorders.GABA is involved in several of the behavioral effects of alcohol,including motor incoordination,anxioly-sis,sedation,withdrawal signs,and ethanol prefer-ence (Buck,1996;Grobin et al .,1998).GABA A receptor agonists tend to potentiate the behavioral e?ects of alcohol,while GABA A receptor antagonists attenuate these e?ects.GABA A receptors have been implicated in ethanol tolerance and dependence (Grobin et al .,1998),and GABA is believed to play a role in central nervous system disinhibition related to the predisposition to alcoholism (Begleiter and Porjesz,1999).GABA A receptors are thought to be involved in beta brain rhythms as measured by the EEG (Porjesz et al .,2002).These studies,taken together with converging linkage evidence on chro-mosome 4,led us to believe that these genes provided strong candidates for their involvement in alcohol dependence and related (endo/)phenotypes.

To systematically explore potential association with these genes,we genotyped 69single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs)within the cluster of four GABA A receptor genes on chromosome 4(Edenberg et al .,2004).Linkage disequilibrium between the SNPs was determined using the program

GOLD

LOD scores for chromosome 4for alcohol dependence diagnoses using multiple diagnostic systems (Williams et al .,1999).

117

COGA Linkage

(Abecasis and Cookson,2000).LD was generally high within genes,and low between genes,allowing us to distinguish between the four tightly clustered GABA A receptor genes.

The Pedigree Disequilibrium test (PDT)(Martin et al .,2000)was used to analyze association with alcohol dependence in the extended COGA pedigrees.The PDT utilizes data from all available trios in a family,as well as discordant sibships.Association analyses with the EEG Beta 2phenotype were carried out using the measured genotype test implemented in SOLAR (Almasy and Blangero,1998).Signi?cant evidence of association with DSM-IV alcohol dependence was observed with 31SNPs in GABRA2,but only 1SNP in the surrounding GABA A receptor genes;signi?cant evidence of association with EEG was observed with 25GABRA2SNPs,but only 1SNP in the ?anking genes (Edenberg et al .,2004).A haplotype comprised of three SNPs in GABRA2that showed signi?cant evidence of association individu-ally with both alcohol dependence and the EEG endophenotype,provided highly signi?cant evi-dence of association with GABRA2.Additionally,

systematic,sliding-window haplotype analyses found 43consecutive three SNP haplotypes that were signi?cant across GABRA2.Signi?cant association between GABRA2and alcohol dependence has sub-sequently been replicated by two independent research groups (Covault et al .,2004;Kranzler et al .,2004;Xu et al .,2004).We are currently investigating functional di?erences associated with genetic variation in GABRA2,in order to determine how this gene is involved in the predisposition to alcohol dependence.Chromosome 7

In the initial Wave 1COGA sample of 105pedigrees,chromosome 7provided the strongest evidence of linkage to alcohol dependence (Reich et al .,1998).Using the COGA criteria for alcohol dependence,the maximum multipoint lod score on chromosome 7was 3.49near the marker D7S1793,using the SIBPHASE option of ASPEX (Hinds and Risch,1999).Subsequently,an additional 10markers were genotyped on chromosome 7.The

multipoint

Fig.3.LOD scores for EEG beta frequencies on chromosome 4(Porjesz et al .,2002).

118Dick et al.

LOD score from sibling pair linkage analyses drop-ped to2.0;however,the Wave2sample of COGA also showed modest,consistent evidence of linkage to chromosome7(Foroud et al.,2000).When the samples were combined for analysis,the maximum lod score shifted to a more distal marker,peaking at 2.9near the marker D7S821(Fig.4).

There was also signi?cant evidence of linkage to electrophysiological endophenotypes on chromo-some7.The event-related theta band of the P300 event related potential showed signi?cant evidence of linkage on chromosome7,with a maximum lod score of3.5between the markers D7S1837and D7S509 (Fig.5)(Jones et al.,2004).The linkage peak observed with the frontal theta band oscillation of the P300ERP was located directly at a muscarinic cholinergic receptor gene,CHRM2.The muscarinic cholinergic receptors are G-protein coupled receptors that in?uence the effects of acetylcholine in the cen-tral and peripheral nervous system;therefore,they are expected to have a direct in?uence on the P300 generation(Frodl-Bauch et al.,1999).The musca-rinic cholinergic receptors are also thought to be involved in cognition and memory(Comings et al., 2003).Recently,signi?cant de?cits in behavioral ?exibility,working memory,and hippocampal plasticity were observed in CHRM2knockout mice (Seeger et al.,2004).These?ndings are relevant be-cause ERP components have been associated with a variety of higher cognitive processes such as object recognition,motor planning,working memory,and inhibitory executive control(Ruchkin et al.,1995). Similarly,speci?c frequency rhythms of oscillatory responses have been attributed to underlie various cognitive processes,with delta involved in signal detection and decision-making(Basar et al.,1999) while theta is involved in conscious awareness, recognition memory,and episodic retrieval(e.g., Klimesch et al.,1994).These observations are con-sistent with a role of CHRM2for theta and delta event-related oscillations.

Accordingly,a small number of SNPs in and around the CHRM2gene were tested in the COGA sample.Signi?cant linkage disequilibrium was ob-served with a SNP in CHRM2for the theta band endophenotype(Jones et al.,2004);inclusion of the SNP as a covariate in the linkage analyses produced an$10%LOD score drop for the theta band phe-notypes,suggesting that this SNP played some functional role in the endophenotype.In addition to our report of linkage disequilibrium between event-related theta band oscillations and CHRM2,

an LOD scores for the COGA de?nition of alcohol dependence on chromosome7(Foroud et al.,2000).

119 COGA Linkage

independent study also reported an association be-tween a polymorphism in CHRM2and major depression in women using a case-control association design (Comings et al .,2002).

These results led us to further explore the evi-dence for linkage on chromosome 7and association with the CHRM2gene,to both alcohol dependence and depression.We typed additional microsatellite markers on chromosome 7and reran linkage analyses using affected sibling pair methods (using the SIB-PHASE option in ASPEX)with the phenotypes COGA alcohol dependence,major depression,alco-holism or major depression (testing the possibility that the gene predisposed to either condition),and alcoholism and major depression (testing the possi-bility that the gene predisposed to the comorbid condition)(Fig.6)(Wang et al .,2004).The peak LOD score for alcohol dependence (488sib pairs,IBD sharing=56.5%)was 2.9observed at D7S1799(note that this was a newly added marker located adjacent to the marker showing the peak lod score with alcohol dependence in the previous analyses of the sample)(Foroud et al .,2000).The peak lod score for depression (259sib pairs,IBD sharing=58.1%)was 2.3,located between D7S1799and D7S1817.The highest lod score was obtained with the phenotype alcoholism or major depression (639sib pairs,IBD sharing=56.2%),resulting in a maximum lod score of 3.4.However,the strongest allele-sharing was observed with the comorbid phenotype alcoholism and depression (144sib pairs,IBD sharing=61%),with a peak lod score of 2.3.Because of the previous evidence of linkage disequilibrium with the ERP phenotype,we conducted family-based association analyses on 11SNPs spanning a 70kb region within and ?anking the CHRM2gene (Wang et al .,2004).There was signi?cant evidence of association with multiple SNPs in CHRM2with both

alcohol

LOD scores on chromosome 7for the target case visual evoked oscillation phenotype.The phenotype data were derived using time-frequency analysis of the event-related potential (ERP)data.Mean values were calculated from time-frequency representations within a time-frequency region of interest corresponding to the P300event (300à700msec)and the theta frequency band (3à7Hz)(Jones et al .,2004).

120Dick et al.

dependence and major depression,and haplotype analyses were also signi?cant with both phenotypes.Interestingly,there was a common protective haplo-type observed for both phenotypes;however,the component alleles of the risk haplotypes differed for each disorder.These analyses provide evidence of both common and speci?c genetic effects on alcohol dependence and depression.Unlike the previous re-port,there was no evidence for sex-speci?c effects in the association results in our sample (Comings et al .,2002).Additional work is on-going to attempt to identify how CHRM2is involved in the risk path-ways for these disorders.DISCUSSION

We believe that these analyses demonstrate that the strategy adopted by the COGA project is suc-cessfully leading to gene identi?cation.Other ?elds have a history of using quantitative risk factors to identify susceptibility genes for disease;for example,cholesterol levels,blood pressure,and body fat have been analyzed to identify genetic risk factors in?uencing cardiovascular disease (Falchi et al .,2004;Ma et al .,2004).The application of this strategy to the study of psychiatric disorders has been more recent.In addition to the alcohol dependence ?eld,the use of endophenotypes has been employed in the study of genetic risk factors for schizophrenia (Clementz,1998).A recent paper re-ported that the analysis of cognitive trait compo-nents of schizophrenia yielded higher lod scores than analyses of diagnosis (Paunio et al .,2004),similar to our experience comparing results from quantitative endophenotypes and diagnostic outcomes in the COGA sample.

In this paper,we have highlighted our systematic efforts focusing on two particular chromosomes where linkage to electrophysiological endopheno-types has led us to signi?cant associations with clin-ical diagnoses.However,we think it is important to note that we are simultaneously employing comple-mentary strategies,including testing for association with candidate genes selected based on hypotheses about biological processes believed to contribute to alcohol dependence.Although some of

these

Fig.6.LOD scores on chromosome 7for the COGA de?nition of alcohol dependence (solid gray line),major depressive syndrome (solid black line),alcohol dependence and major depressive syndrome (dashed black line),and alcohol dependence or major depressive syndrome (dashed gray line)in the combined COGA sample (Wang et al .,2004).

121

COGA Linkage

candidate genes are located in regions that show little evidence of linkage,we recognize that linkage studies are generally underpowered to detect genes of small effect and that association analyses may provide a more powerful test.Employing a hypothesis-driven approach based upon information about biological systems likely to be involved in alcohol dependence has also led us to genes that signi?cantly in?uence alcohol dependence in our sample(Dick et al.,2004). In addition,we are currently following up chromo-somal regions that show linkage to either clinical phenotypes or endophenotypes(but not necessarily both).One disadvantage of working with endophe-notypes is that the number of traits available for analysis can create problems associated with multiple testing.These can generally be addressed through simulation;however,it can be unclear how to inter-pret linkage/association signals speci?c to the end-ophenotype that show no correlation with linkage/ association signals related to clinical diagnoses.These ?ndings may be informative for advancing our understanding of the underlying processes involved in the electrophysiological signals;however,our most successful e?orts to date in COGA have been on chromosomes showing evidence of linkage with both electrophysiological endophenotypes and clinical diagnoses.

The goal of this paper has been to collate and summarize a series of analyses conducted over the course of the COGA project that,we believe,raise a number of interesting points regarding the use of endophenotypes in gene-identi?cation efforts.Our experience thus far has been that the linkage peaks observed with endophenotypes have been located directly over the genes subsequently found to be associated with alcohol dependence;alcohol depen-dence diagnoses have not yielded linkage peaks at the location of the(currently identi?ed)genes.On both chromosome4and chromosome7,although there was linkage to both clinical diagnoses and to elec-trophysiological endophenotypes,in neither case were the linkage peaks located at the same position. Two scenarios could create this situation:(1)the linkage evidence from electrophysiological endophe-notypes and clinical diagnoses could result from dif-ferent genes or(2)there could be random variation in the location of the linkage peak,although both linkage signals are detecting the same genetic effect. Simulation studies have demonstrated considerable variation in the linkage peaks observed for complex phenotypes with sample sizes comparable to that of COGA(Roberts et al.,1999).

On chromosome4the peaks observed for alco-hol dependence/alcohol-related phenotypes and EEG were$50cM apart.Analyses of the quantitative phenotypes(max drinks and EEG)clearly suggest two linkage peaks on chromosome4:one near the GABA A receptor gene cluster and another near the ADH gene cluster.Alternatively,analyses of alcohol dependence diagnoses show only one very broad peak,with the one-lod interval stretching nearly 50cM and the two-lod interval stretching nearly 100cM!Importantly,we note that although GA-BRA2shows signi?cant association with alcohol dependence,it was ultimately the strong linkage sig-nal observed directly over the GABA A receptor gene cluster with the EEG endophenotypes,as well as strong evidence from the literature suggesting that GABA reception is involved in the predisposition to alcohol dependence,that led us to focus on the GABA A receptor genes on chromosome4.The lod score observed with dependence diagnoses directly at the location of the GABA A receptor gene cluster was <1.We are currently conducting association analy-ses with the ADH genes on chromosome4,which are located very near to the linkage peaks observed for dependence diagnoses and for max drinks.We?nd signi?cant association with genes in that region as well(Edenberg et al.,in preparation).It is possible that the wide region of linkage observed with the clinical diagnoses re?ected the presence of multiple genes on chromosome4in?uencing alcohol depen-dence,in both the GABA and ADH gene regions. Only the quantitative phenotypes distinguished two distinct peaks in the region.

On chromosome7the linkage peaks for alco-holism and depression are located$30cM centro-meric to the ERP linkage peak.The CHRM2gene is located directly under the linkage peak observed with the ERP phenotype.Although CHRM2showed sig-ni?cant association with both alcohol dependence and major depression,the gene lies outside of the1lod support interval of the linkage peaks observed with the diagnostic phenotypes.In general,our experience thus far is that the linkage peaks associated with the endophenotypes have been narrower and have been located directly over the gene subsequently found to be associated with both the endophenotype and with clinical diagnosis.We believe it is likely that there are additional genes on chromosome7that in?uence the development of alcohol dependence.Approximately 15cM distal of the alcoholism peak(and14cM proximal to the CHRM2gene)lies a gene coding for the TAS2R16taste receptor,identi?ed as a bitter taste

122Dick et al.

receptor for beta glucopyranosides(such as salicin) (Bufe et al.,2002).

A functional polymorphism in this gene appears to increase susceptibility to develop alcohol depen-dence in the COGA sample(Hinrichs et al.,2005). We have tested SNPs in a number of additional candidate genes located more directly under the alcohol dependence/depression linkage peaks,al-though none have yielded the signi?cant association results observed with CHRM2thus far.We are cur-rently planning a systematic screen of SNPs located across the alcohol dependence peak on chromosome 7.We believe that this is necessary to more de?ni-tively determine whether there are other genes in the region contributing to the alcohol dependence link-age signal.The results from these analyses should help us determine whether CHRM2was the only gene contributing to the linkage observed with alcohol dependence and the localization was simply poor,or whether the discrepancy between the location of the linkage peaks for ERP and alcohol dependence was due to additional gene(s)being detected by the diagnostic phenotypes.

In conclusion,the decision to measure and in-clude electrophysiological endophenotypes in addi-tion to clinical diagnoses in the COGA project has played a critical role in our gene identi?cation efforts. These endophenotypes have successfully led to the identi?cation of GABRA2and CHRM2as genes associated with alcohol dependence.Alcohol depen-dence diagnoses yielded evidence of linkage on both chromosome4and chromosome7,the chromosomes on which these genes are located,respectively;how-ever,in neither case was the peak observed with the dependence diagnoses located at these genes.How-ever,there were narrow,signi?cant linkage peaks obtained with the electrophysiological endopheno-types directly at the location of these genes.In addition to the sharp resolution provided by the endophenotypes,the linkage signals have generally been stronger with these quantitative traits as com-pared to signals obtained from analyses of depen-dence diagnoses.As an example,on chromosome4, the peak lod score obtained for alcohol dependence was2.8.However,the EEG linkage peak maximized at6.53,clearly surpassing the threshold for signi?-cance suggested by Lander and Kruglyak(Lander and Kruglyak,1995).We have generally not found linkage peaks associated with dependence diagnoses that reach the level of signi?cance suggested by Lander and Kruglyak(Lander and Kruglyak,1995), even in chromosomal regions where genes have subsequently been identi?ed.Thus,the use of end-ophenotypes has been advantageous in advancing our understanding of genetic contributions to alcohol dependence in a number of ways.In addition to being informative about the processes likely to be involved in the predisposition to alcohol dependence,they have proven very useful in aiding gene identi?cation e?orts for alcohol dependence. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism(COGA)(Principal Investigator: H.Begleiter;Co-Principal Investigators:L.Bierut, H.Edenberg,V.Hesselbrock,B.Porjesz)includes nine different centers where data collection,analysis, and storage take place.The nine sites and Principal Investigators and Co-Investigators are:University of Connecticut(V.Hesselbrock);Indiana University (H.Edenberg,J.Nurnberger Jr.,P.M.Conneally, T.Foroud);University of Iowa(S.Kuperman, R.Crowe);SUNY HSCB(B.Porjesz,H.Begleiter); Washington University in St.Louis(L.Bierut, A.Goate,J.Rice);University of California at San Diego(M.Schuckit);Howard University(R.Taylor); Rutgers University(J.Tisch?eld);Southwest Foun-dation(L.Almasy).Zhaoxia Ren serves as the NIAAA Staff Collaborator.This national collabo-rative study is supported by the NIH Grant U10AA08401from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism(NIAAA)and the National Institute on Drug Abuse(NIDA).In memory of Theodore Reich,M.D.,Co-Principal Investigator of COGA since its inception and one of the founders of modern psychiatric genetics,we acknowledge his immeasurable and fundamental scienti?c contribu-tions to COGA and the?eld.

The?rst author DMD would also like to thank Dr.Irving Gottesman for comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript,and more generally,for his invaluable mentorship.

REFERENCES

Abecasis,G.R.,and Cookson,W.O.(2000).GOLDàGraphical overview of linkage disequilibrium.Bioinformatics16:182à183. Almasy,L.,and Blangero,J.(1998).Multipoint quantitative-trait linkage analysis in general pedigrees.Am.J.Hum.Genet.

62:1198à1211.

Almasy,L.,Porjesz,B.,Blangero,J.,Chorlian,D.B.,O’Connor,S.

J.,Kuperman,S.,Rohrbaugh,J.,Bauer,L.O.,Reich,T., Polich,J.,and Begleiter,H.(1999).Heritability of event-re-lated brain potentials in families with a history of alcoholism.

Am.J.Med.Genet.88:383à390.

123

COGA Linkage

Almasy,L.,Porjesz,B.,Blangero,J.,Goate,A.,Edenberg,H.J., Chorlian,D.B.,Kuperman,S.,O’Connor,S.J.,Rohrbaugh, J.,Bauer,L.O.,Foroud,T.,Rice,J.,Reich,T.,and Begleiter,

H.(2001).Genetics of event-related brain potential in response

to a semantic priming paradign in families with a history of alcoholism.Am.J.Hum.Genet.68:128à135.

Basar, E.,Basar-Eroglu, C.,Karakas,S.,and Schumann,M.

(1999).Are cognitive processes manifested in event-related gamma,alpha,theta and delta oscillations in the EEG?Neu-rosci.Lett.259:165à168.

Begleiter,H.,and Porjesz,B.(1999).What is inherited in the pre-disposition toward alcoholism?A proposed model.Alcohol.

Clin.Exp.Res.23:1125à1135.

Begleiter,H.,Porjesz,B.,Bihari,B.,and Kissin,B.(1984).Event-related potentials in boys at risk for alcoholism.Science 225:1493à1496.

Begleiter,H.,Porjesz,B.,Reich,T.,Edenberg,H.J.,Goate,A., Blangero,J.,Almasy,L.,Foroud,T.,Van Eerdewegh,P., Polich,J.,Rohrbaugh,J.,Kuperman,S.,Bauer,L.O., O’Connor,S.J.,Chorlian,D.B.,Li,T.K.,Conneally,P.M., Hesselbrock,V.,Rice,J.,Schuckit,M.,Cloninger, C.R., Nurnberger,J.I.Jr.,Crowe,R.,and Bloom,F.E.(1998).

Quantitative trait loci analysis of human event-related brain potentials:P3voltage.Electroencephalogr.Clin.Neurophysiol.

108:244à250.

Bucholz,K.K.,Cadoret,R.,Cloninger,C.R.,Dinwiddie,S.H., Hesselbrock,V.M.,Nurnberger,J.I.Jr.,Reich,T.,Schmidt,

I.,and Schuckit,M.A.(1994).A new,semi-structured psy-

chiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies:A report on the reliability of the SSAGA.J.Stud.Alcohol55:149à158. Buck,K.J.(1996).Molecular genetic analysis of the role of GABAergic systems in the behavioral and cellular actions of alcohol.Behav.Genet.26:313à323.

Bufe,B.,Hofmann,T.,Krautwurst,D.,Raguse,J.D.,and Mey-erhof,W.(2002).The human TAS2R16receptor mediates bitter taste in response to beta-glucopyranosides.Nat.Genet.

32:397à401.

Carlson,S.R.,Iacono,W.G.,and McGue,M.(2004).P300 amplitude in nonalcoholic adolescent twin pairs who become discordant for alcoholism as adults.Psychophysiology 41:841à844.

Clementz, B. A.(1998).Psychophysiological measures of (dis)inhibition as liability indicators for schizophrenia.Psy-chophysiology35:648à668.

Cloninger, C.R.(1987).Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism.Science236:410à416.

Comings,D.E.,Wu,S.,Rostamkhani,M.,McGue,M.,Iacono, W.G.,Cheng,L.S.,and MacMurray,J.P.(2003).Role of the cholinergic muscarinic2receptor(CHRM2)gene in cognition.

Mol.Psychiatry8:10à13.

Comings,D.E.,Wu,S.,Rostamkhani,M.,McGue,M.,Iacono, W.G.,and MacMurray,J.P.(2002).Association of the muscarinic cholinergic2receptor(CHRM2)gene with major depression in women.Am.J.Med.Genet.114:527à529. Covault,J.,Gelernter,J.,Hesselbrock,V.,Nellissery,M.,and Kranzler,H.R.(2004).Allelic and haplotypic association of GABRA2with alcohol dependence.Am.J.Med.Genet.

(Neuropsychiatr.Genet.)129B:104à109.

de Geus,E.J.,and Boomsma,D.I.(2001).A genetic neuroscience approach to human cognition.Eur.Psychol.6:241à253. Dick,D.M.,Edenberg,H.J.,Xuei,X.,Goate,A.,Kuperman,S., Schuckit,M.,Crowe,R.,Smith,T.L.,Porjesz,B.,Begleiter,

H.,and Foroud,T.(2004).Association of GABRG3with

alcohol dependence.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.28:4à9.

Dick,D.M.,Nurnberger,J.I.Jr.,Edenberg,H.J.,Goate,A., Crowe,R.,Rice,J.,Hesselbrock,V.,Bucholz,K.K.,Kra-mer,J.R.,Porjesz,B.,Begleiter,H.,and Foroud,T.(2002).

Suggestive linkage on chromosome1for a quantitative

alcohol/personality phenotype.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.

26:1453à1460.

Dick,D.M.,Rose,R.J.,Viken,R.J.,Kaprio,J.,and Koskenvuo, M.(2001).Exploring gene-environment interactions:Sociore-gional moderation of alcohol use.J.Abnorm.Psychol.

110:625à632.

Edenberg,H.J.,Dick,D.M.,Xuei,X.,Tian,H.,Almasy,L., Bauer,L.O.,Crowe,R.,Goate,A.,Hesselbrock,V.,Jones,K.

A.,Kwon,J.,Li,T.K.,Nurnberger,J.I.Jr.,O’Connor,S.J.,

Reich,T.,Rice,J.,Schuckit,M.,Porjesz,B.,Foroud,T.,and Begleiter,H.(2004).Variations in GABRA2,encoding the a2 subunit of the GABA-A receptor are associated with alcohol dependence and with brain oscillations.Am.J.Hum.Genet.

74:705à714.

Egeland,J.A.,Gerhard,D.S.,Pauls,D.L.,Sussex,J.N.,Kidd, K.K.,Allen,C.R.,Hostetter,A.M.,and Housman,D.E.

(1987).Bipolar a?ective disorders linked to DNA markers on chromosome11.Nature325:783à787.

Falchi,M.,Forabosco,P.,Mocci,E.,Borlino,C.C.,Picciau,A., Virdis,E.,Persico,I.,Parracciani,D.,Anguis,A.,and Pirastu, M.(2004).A genomewide search using an original pairwise sampling approach for large geneaologies identi?es a new locus for total and low-density lipoprotein cholesterol in two genetically di?erntiated isolates of Sardinia.Am.J.Hum.

Genet.75:1015à1031.

Ferreira,M.A.R.(2004).Linkage analysis:Principles and meth-ods for the analysis of human quantitative traits.Twin Res.

7:513à530.

Finn,P.R.,Sharkansky,E.J.,Viken,R.,West,T.L.,Sandy, J.,and Bu?erd,G.M.(1997).Heterogeneity in the families of sons of alcoholics:The impact of familial vulnerability type on o?spring characteristics.J.Abnorm.Psychol.

106:26à36.

Foroud,T.,Bucholz,K.K.,Edenberg,H.J.,et al.(1998).Linkage of an alcoholism-related severity phenotype to chromosome

16.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.22(9):2035à2042

Foroud,T.,Edenberg,H.J.,Goate,A.,Rice,J.,Flury,L.,Koller,

D.L.,Bierut,L.J.,Conneally,P.M.,Nurnberger,J.I.,

Bucholz,K.K.,Li,T.-K.,Hesselbrock,V.,Crowe,R., Schuckit,M.,Porjesz,B.,Begleiter,H.,and Reich,T.(2000).

Alcoholism susceptibility loci:Con?rmation studies in a rep-licate sample and further mapping.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.

24:933à945.

Frodl-Bauch,T.,Bottlender,R.,and Hegerl,U.(1999).Neuro-chemical substrates and neuroanatomical generators of the event-related P300.Neuropsychobiology40:86à94. Gershon,E.S.,DeLisi,L.E.,Hamovit,J.,Nurnberger,J.I.Jr., Maxwell,M.E.,Schreiber,J.,Dauphinais,D.,Dingman,C.

W.,and Guro?,J.J.(1988).A controlled family study of chronic psychoses.Arch.Gen.Psychiatry45:328à336. Ghosh,S.,Begleiter,H.,Porjesz,B.,Chorlian,D.B.,Edenberg,

H.J.,Foroud,T.,Goate,A.,and Reich,T.(2003).Linkage

mapping of beta2EEG waves via non-parametric regression.

Am.J.Med.Genet.(Neuropsychiatr.Genet.)118B:66à71. Gottesman,I.I.,and Gould,T.D.(2003).The endophenotype concept in psychiatry:Etymology and strategic intentions.

Am.J.Psychiatry160:1à10.

Gottesman,I.I.,and Shields,J.(1972).Schizophrenia and Genetics:

A Twin Study Vantage Point.London:Academic.

Grobin,A.C.,Matthews,D.B.,Devaud,L.L.,and Morrow,A.L.

(1998).The role of GABA-A receptors in the acute and chronic e?ects of ethanol.Psychopharmacology139:2à19. Heath,A.C.(1995).Genetic in?uences on drinking behavior in humans.In H.Begleiter and B.Kissin(eds.),The Genetics of Alcoholism.New York:Oxford University Press,pp.82à121. Heath,A.C.,Jardine,R.,and Martin,N.G.(1989).Interactive e?ects of genotype and social environment on alcohol con-sumption in female twins.J.Stud.Alcohol50:38à48.

124Dick et al.

Hesselbrock,M.,Easton,C.,Bucholz,K.K.,Schuckit,M.,and Hesselbrock,V.(1999).A validity study of the SSAGA-A comparison with the SCAN.Addiction94:1361à1370. Hinds,D.and Risch,N.(1999).The ASPEX package:A?ected sib-pair exclusion mapping.

Hinrichs,A.L.,Wang,J.C.,Bufe,B.,Kwon,J.M.,Budde,J., Allen,R.,Bertelsen,S.,Evans,W.,Dick,D.M.,Rice,J., Foroud,T.,Nurnberger Jr.,J.I.,Tisch?eld,J.A.,Kuperman, S.,Crowe,R.,Hesselbrock,V.,Schuckit,M.,Almasy,L., Porjesz,B.,Edenberg,H.J.,Begleiter,H.,Meyerhof,W., Bierut,L.,and Goate,A.M.(2005).Functional variant in a bitter taste receptor(hTAS2R16)in?uences risk for alcohol dependence.Amer.J.Human Genet.(in press).

Jones,K.A.,Porjesz,B.,Almasy,L.,Bierut,L.,Goate,A.,Wang, J. C.,Dick, D.M.,Hinrichs, A.L.,Kwon,J.,Rice,J., Rohrbaugh,J.,Stock,H.,Wu,W.,Bauer,L.O.,Chorlian,D.

B.,Crowe,R.R.,Edenberg,H.J.,Foroud,T.,Hesselbrock,

V.,Kuperman,S.,Nurnberger,J.I.Jr.,O’Connor,S.J., Schuckit,M.,Stimus,A.,Tisch?eld,J.A.,Reich,T.,and Begleiter,H.(2004).Linkage and linkage disequilibrium of evoked EEG oscillations with CHRM2receptor gene poly-morphisms:Implications for human brain dynamics and cognition.Int.J.Psychophysiol.53:75à90.

Kelsoe,J.R.,Ginns,E.I.,Egeland,J.A.,Gerhard,D.S.,Gold-stein,A.M.,Bale,S.J.,Pauls,D.L.,Long,R.T.,Kidd,K.

K.,Conte,G.,et al.(1989).Re-evaluation of the linkage relationship between chromosome11p loci and the gene for bipolar a?ective disorder in the Old Order Amish.Nature 342:238à243

Klimesch,W.,Schimke,H.,and Schwaiger,J.(1994).Episodic and semantic memory:An anlysis in the EEG theta and alpha band.Electroencephalogr.Clin.Neurophysiol.91:428à441. Koopmans,J.R.,Slutske,W.S.,van Baal,G. C.M.,and Boomsma,D.I.(1999).The in?uence of religion on alcohol use initiation:Evidence for genotype x environment interac-tion.Behav.Genet.29:445à453.

Kranzler,H.R.,Covault,J.,Gelernter,J.,and Nellissery,M.

(2004).Allelic and haplotypic association of GABA alpha-2 gene with alcohol dependence.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.28:49A .

Lander,E.,and Kruglyak,L.(1995).Genetic dissection of complex traits:Guidelines for interpreting and reporting linkage re-sults.Nat.Genet.11:241à247.

Long,J.C.,Knowler,W.C.,Hanson,R.L.,et al.(1998).Evidence for genetic linkage to alcohol dependence on chromosomes4 and11from an autosome-wide scan in an American Indian population.Am.J.Med.Genet.81:216à221

Ma,X.,Bacci,X.,Milynarski,W.,Gottardo,L.,Soccio,T., Menzaghi,C.,Iori,E.,Lager,R.A.,Shro?,A.R.,Gervino,

E.V.,Nesto,R.W.,Johnstone,M.T.,Abumrad,N.A.,

Avogaro,A.,Trischitta,V.,and Doria,A.(2004).A common haplotype at the CD36locus is associated with high free fatty acide levels and increased cardiovascular risk in Caucasians.

Hum.Mol.Genet.13:2197à2205.

Martin,E.R.,Monks,S.A.,Warren,L.L.,and Kaplan,N.L.

(2000).A test for linkage and association in general pedigrees: The Pedigree Disequilibrium Test.Am.J.Hum.Genet.

67:146à154.

McGue,M.(1999).The behavioral genetics of alcoholism.Curr.

Direct.Psychol.Sci.8:109à115.

Paunio,T.,Tuulio-Henriksson,A.,Hiekkalinna,T.,Perola,M., Varilo,T.,Partonen,T.,Cannon,T.D.,Lonnqvist,J.,and Peltonen,L.(2004).Search for cognitive trait components of schizophrenia reveals a locus for verbal learning and memory on4q and for visual working memory on2q.Hum.Mol.

Genet.15:1693à1702.

Porjesz,B.,Almasy,L.,Edenberg,H.J.,Wang,K.,Chorlian,D.

B.,Foroud,T.,Goate,A.,Rice,J.,O’Connor,S.J.,Rohrb-

augh,J.,Kuperman,S.,Bauer,L.O.,Crowe,R.,Schuckit,

M.,Hesselbrock,V.,Conneally,P.M.,Tisch?eld,J.A.,Li,T.

K.,Reich,T.,and Begleiter,H.(2002).Linkage disequilibrium between the beta frequency of the human EEG and a GABAA receptor gene locus.PNAS99:3729à3733.

Porjesz, B.,and Begleiter,H.(2003).Alcoholism and human electrophysiology.Alcohol Res.Health27:153à160. Porjesz,B.,Jones,K.,and Begleiter,H.(2004).The genetics of oscillations in the human brain.Proc.Clin.Neurophysiol.

57:437à445.

Porjesz,B.,Rangaswamy,M.,Kamarajan,C.,Jones,K.,Pad-manabhapillai,A.,and Begleiter,H.(2005).The utility of neurophysiological markers in the study of alcoholism.Clin.

Neurophysiol.116:993à1018.

Rangaswamy,M.,Porjesz,B.,Ardekani,B.A.,Choi,S.J.,Tan-abe,J.L.,Lim,K.O.,and Begleiter,H.(2004a).A functional MRI study of visual oddball:Evidence for frontoparietal dysfunction in subjects at risk for alcoholism.Neuroimage 21:329à339.

Rangaswamy,M.,Porjesz,B.,Chorlian,D.B.,Choi,K.,Jones,K., Wang,K.,Rohrbaugh,J.,O’Connor,S.J.,Kuperman,S., Reich,T.,and Begleiter,H.(2003).Theta power in the EEG of alcoholics.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.27:607à615. Rangaswamy,M.,Porjesz,B.,Chorlian,D.B.,Wang,K.,Jones, K.A.,Bauer,L.O.,Rohrbaugh,J.,O’Connor,S.J.,Kuper-man,S.,Reich,T.,and Begleiter,H.(2002).Beta power in the EEG of alcoholics.Biol.Psychiatry52:831à842. Rangaswamy,M.,Porjesz,B.,Chorlian,D.B.,Wang,K.,Jones, K.A.,Kuperman,S.,Rohrbaugh,J.,O’Connor,S.J.,Bauer, L.O.,Reich,T.,and Begleiter,H.(2004b).Resting EEG in o?spring of male alcoholics:beta frequencies.Int.J.Psycho-physiol.51:239à251.

Reese,C.,and Polich,J.(2003).Alcoholism risk and the P300 event-related brain potential:modality,task,and gender ef-fects.Brain Cogn.53:46à57.

Reich,T.(1996).A genomic survey of alcohol dependence and related phenotypes:Results from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism(COGA).Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.

20:133Aà137A.

Reich,T.,Edenberg,H.J.,Goate,A.,Williams,J.T.,Rice,J.P., Van Eerdewegh,P.,Foroud,T.,Hesselbrock,V.,Schuckit,M.

A.,Bucholz,K.,Porjesz,

B.,Li,T.K.,Conneally,P.M.,

Nurnberger,J.I.Jr.,Tisch?eld,J.A.,Crowe,R.R.,Clonin-ger,C.R.,Wu,W.,Shears,S.,Carr,K.,Crose,C.,Willig,C., and Begleiter,H.(1998).Genome-wide search for genes a?ecting the risk for alcohol dependence.Am.J.Med.Genet.

81:207à215.

Roberts,S.B.,MacLean,C.J.,Neale,M.C.,Eaves,L.J.,and Kendler,K.S.(1999).Replication of linkage studies of com-plex traits:An examination of variation in location estimates.

Am.J.Hum.Genet.65:876à884.

Rose,R.J.,Dick,D.M.,Viken,R.J.,and Kaprio,J.(2001a).

Gene-environment interaction in patterns of adolescent drinking:Regional residency moderates longitudinal in?u-ences on alcohol use.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.25:637à643. Rose,R.J.,Dick,D.M.,Viken,R.J.,Pulkkinen,L.,and Kaprio, J.(2001b).Drinking or abstaining at age14:A genetic epi-demiological study.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.Res.25:1594à1604. Rose,R.J.,Viken,R.J.,Dick,D.M.,Bates,J.,Pulkkinen,L.,and Kaprio,J.(2003).It does take a village:Nonfamilial envi-ronments and children’s behavior.Psychol.Sci.14:273à277. Ruchkin,D.S.,ACanoune,H.L.,Johnson,R.,and Ritter,O.

(1995).Working memory and preparation elicit di?erent pat-terns of slow wave event-related brain potentials.Psycho-physiology32:399à410.

Saccone,N.,Kwon,J.M.,Corbett,J.,Goate,A.,Rochberg,N., Edenberg,H.J.,Foroud,T.,Li,T.K.,Begleiter,H.,Reich,T., and Rice,J.P.(2000).A genome screen of maximum number of drinks as an alcoholism phenotype.Neuropsychiatr.Genet.

96:632à637.

125

COGA Linkage

Seeger,T.,Fedorova,I.,Zheng,F.,Miyakawa,T.,Koustova,E., Gomeza,J.,Basile,A.S.,Alzheimer,C.,and Wess,J.(2004).

M2muscarinic acetylcholine receptor knock-out mice show de?cits in behavioral?exibility,working memory,and hip-pocampal plasticity.J.Neurosci.24:10117à10127.

Song,J.,Koller,D.L.,Foroud,T.,Rice,J.,Nurnberger,J.I.Jr., Begleiter,H.,Porjesz,B.,Smith,T.L.,Schuckit,M.,and Edenberg,H.J.(2003).Association of GABA-A receptors and alcohol dependence and the e?ects of genetic imprinting.Am.

J.Med.Genet.117B:39à45.

van Beijsterveldt,C.E.,Molenaar,P.C.,de Geus,E.J.,and Boomsma,D.I.(1996).Heritability of human brain func-tioning as assessed by electroencephalography.Am.J.Hum.

Genet.58:562à573.

Wang,J.C.,Hinrichs,A.L.,Stock,H.,Budde,J.,Allen,R., Bertelsen,S.,Kwon,J.M.,Wu,W.,Dick,D.M.,Jones,K., Nurnberger,J.I.Jr.,Tisch?eld,J.A.,Porjesz,B.,Edenberg,

H.J.,Hesselbrock,V.,Crowe,R.,Schuckit,M.,Begleiter,H.,

Reich,T.,Goate, A.,and Bierut,L.(2004).Evidence of

common and speci?c genetic e?ects:Association of the mus-carinic acetylcholine receptor M2(CHRM2)gene with alcohol dependence and major depressive syndrome.Hum.Mol.

Genet.13:1903à1911.

Williams,J.T.,Begleiter,H.,Porjesz,B.,Edenberg,H.J.,Foroud, T.,Reich,T.,Goate,A.,Van Eerdewegh,P.,Almasy,L.,and Blangero,J.(1999).Joint multipoint linkage analysis of mul-tivariate qualitative and quantitative traits.II.Alcoholism and event-related potentials.Am.J.Hum.Genet.65:1148à1160. Xu,K.,Westly,E.,Taubman,J.,astor,W.,Lipsky,R.H.,and Goldman, D.(2004).Linkage disequilibrium relationships among GABRA cluster genes located on chromosome4with alcohol dependence in two populations.Alcohol.Clin.Exp.

Res.28:48A.

Yordanova,J.,and Kolev,V.(1996).Brain theta response predicts P300latency in children.Neuroreport8:277à280.

Edited by Dorret Boomsma

126Dick et al.

副词用法及练习

副词用法及练习 副词的基本用法是用来修饰动词;run into the room quickly carefully 2). 副 词的构成: (1)多数形容词+ly变成副词 . 大多数副词以ly结尾 recent 最近的----recently 最近;近来 sad—sadly slow---slowly clear—clearly清楚地final—finally real—really kind- - kindly, careful--- carefully, Silent----silently peacefu---peacefully Safe---safely (2)以辅音字母加y结尾的形容词需改y为i加ly变成副词happy-- happily, heavy-- heavily, easy-- easily, lucky--- luckily, angy---angrily, (3.)形容词不去e加ly变来的副词polite--politely, wide--widely, Safe--safely, extreme-- extremely (4 形容词需去e加ly变来的副词True - Truly, , gentle ---gently, comfortable-- comfortably possible---possibly simple --simply terrible---terribly 特殊:good(形容词)—well(副词)好地 (welladj 身体健康的) 1.副词的比较等级,副词的比较等级用法和形容词的比较等级用法相同 以ly的副词大多在前面加more构成比较级加, most构成最高级 slowly– more slowly- most slowly c arefully-more c arefully most c arefully 易错:early →earlier →earliest badly →worse →worst 基础练习选词填空 (一)luckily / lucky / unluchk / unluckily 1. that was an hoy .he lost his parents in the earthquake 2. it rained hard. The children took umbrollas so they didin’t get wet 3.. he is to pass the exam 4. A terrible car accident happpened to him he didn’t lose hos life (二)easy easily easier more easyly 1.If you have more knowledge .you can deal with the problems 2. the question looks I ‘m sure I can woerk out it by meself 3. In generally’it is to say than to do (三)success successful successfully succeed 1. Don’t worry . I heard that the airplne has landed at the airport 2.if you become people will watch you all the time 3. if you try your best you will 4. Attitud is everything Details (细节)decide (四)happy happily happier more happily 1. Because of having parents’ love. I can grow up 2. look . the children are playing over there 3. the busier he is .the he feel (五)safe safely safety 1. School is the serious problem 2. please drive more slowly. is the first 3. Although we were in trouble . we got there 4. To be is important to everyone (六)careful carefullly 1. Liu ming is . girl . she does everything .

possibility的用法总结大全

possibility的用法总结大全 possibility的意思 n. 可能,可能性,希望,可能发生的事物 possibility用法 possibility可以用作名词 possibility后不能接动词不定式,可接“of sth/ v -ing”结构或that同位语从句。 possibility用作名词的用法例句 I see great possibilities in the scheme.我看这计划很可能成功。 We must accept the possibility that we might be wrong.我们必须承认,我们有可能搞错了。 She saw the possibilities of the scheme from the beginning.她从一开始就预见到这计划可能成功。 possibility用法例句 1、There is the possibility that hypothermia can go unrecognized. 人们有可能察觉不到体温的过低。 2、There is the strong possibility that such cooperation will prove unworkable. 这种合作很有可能会行不通。 3、He envisages the possibility of establishing direct diplomatic relations in the future. 他设想在未来可以建立直接的外交关系。 possibility双语例句 1.I stand back, excruciated by the possibility.我向后退了一步,为可能发生的事情烦躁不安。 2.There is one possibility of changing jobs.换工作是有可能的事。 3.Does myopia have resumptive possibility 300 degrees?近视300度有恢复的可能性吗? 4.There is no possibility of his coming.他不可能来。 5.That's a possibility that can't be ruled out.那是一个不能不予考虑的可能性。

careful的用法和短语例句

careful的用法和短语例句 careful有小心谨慎的;慎重的;精心的等意思,那么你知道careful的用法吗?下面跟着小编一起来学习一下,希望对大家的学习有所帮助! careful的用法: careful的用法1:careful的基本意思是仔细的小心的,含有靠认真实践来避免错误的意味。 careful的用法2:careful和about连用,通常后接指物的名词、动名词或从句。 careful的用法3:careful用作表语时,其后也可跟动词不定式、that/wh-从句。 careful的用法4:careful和of连用,通常后接指人或事物的名词,也可接从句。接从句时,of常省略。 careful的常用短语: 用作形容词(adj.) careful about careful for careful of careful的用法例句: 1. You have to be careful what you say on telly.

在电视上说话时你必须很谨慎。 2. Interior decoration by careful coordination seems to have had its day. 精心搭配的室内装饰似乎已不再受欢迎了。 3. One of the keys to successful business is careful planning. 打造成功企业的关键之一就是要精心策划。 4. This very careful attitude to money can sometimes border on meanness. 这种对待金钱的过分谨慎的态度有时几近于吝啬。 5. The British Government was careful not to act unilaterally. 英国政府小心行事,避免单边采取行动。 6. The United States has to be careful it doesnt overplay its hand. 美国必须谨防高估自身实力。 7. It would force industries to be more careful with natural resources. 这将迫使企业更加节约资源。 8. Despite careful detective work, many items have never been recovered. 尽管进行了悉心探查,许多物品还是一直没有找回。

(完整版)非谓语的用法总结

非谓语动词的用法总结 李靖 非谓语动词(不定式、动名词、分词)不是真正意义上的谓语动词,在句中都不能单独作谓语。但都具有动词的某些特点,可以有自己的宾语或状语,构成非谓语动词短语。不定式表目的,表将来;动名词表主动,表进行;过去分词表被动,表完成。 一.作主语( 动名词、不定式、过去分词前面加being, 动名词做主语谓语动词用单数) 1)不定式、动名词都可做主语,但是动名词做主语多指抽象的概念性的动作,可以是多次的经常的行为;不定式多表示具体的动作,尤其是某一次的动作。如: Playing with fire is dangerous. 玩火危险。(泛指玩火) To play with fire will be dangerous. 玩火会发生危险。 2)用动名词做主语的句型: It’s no use/no good/pleasure doing sth 做.......没有用处/好处/乐趣 It’s a waste of time doing sth 做.... 浪费时间 It is worth doing sth 值得做某事 It is no use crying over spilt milk. 覆水难收 It is worth making an appointment before you go. 去之前预约一下是值得的。 3)不定式做主语的句型. It is + adj +for sb to do sth. 比较:表示人的品质时只能用of sb 如considerate/kind/nice/foolish/stupid/rude/cruel /wise/clever/brave/selfish/crazy/good/careful/careless/impolite/right/wrong等单词出现 时。 It is silly of you to say so. It is important for us to learn English well. It is a must/ a necessity for us to have a good command of the English language. 4) “Wh- + to 不定式”可做主语。如: When to leave hasn’t been decided yet.什么时候动身还没定呢。 Whether to drive or take the train is still a problem.是自驾车还是乘火车仍是一个问题。 5)一般情况下,不定式和动名词作主语,谓语动词一般用单数形式。如: Seeing is believing.= To see is to believe 眼见为实。 . Planting flowers needs constant watering. 但and连接的多个动名词作主语,谓语动词大多用复数(如果前后动名词的语义一致, 谓语一般用单数)。如:Lying and stealing are immoral. 说谎与盗窃是不道德的。 6) 动名词前面可以加上形容词性物主代词构成动名词的复合结构。如: ----- What made him angry? ------ Mary’s /My/His/Her /Their/ The boy’s/ The president’s being late(made him angry. 7being,构成动名词,做主语。如: . Being examined twice a year , driver must obey in this city. 经典练习:

最新success用法专项

例题: 1 He took a second driving test and finally ________. 2 A.succeeded in passing it B. succeeded in it C. succeeded 3 to through D. succeeded to pass it. 4 5 succeed in + doing sth = 成功做成某事 6 succeed to ...: 继承(王位等) 7 succeed sb: 继承 8 succeed with sth:在某方面获得成功 9 succeed,success,successful的区别和用法 10 11 一、succeed的用法 12 ◆他的计划成功了。 13 误:His plan was succeeded. 14 正:His plan succeeded. 15 析:succeed 表示“成功”,是不及物动词,因此不能用于被动语态。 16 17 ◆他终于把那个问题解决了。 18

误:At last he succeeded to solve the problem. 19 正:At last he succeeded in solving the problem. 20 析:表示做某事做成功了,succeed 后通常不接不定式,而接in doing sth。 21 又如:He succeeded in getting the job.(他谋到了那份工作), 22 She succeeded in (passing) the exam.(她考试及格了)。 23 注:有时此结构可用作反语,如:I tried to 24 clean the watch, but only succeeded in breaking it. 25 (我想把表弄干净,结果却弄坏了)。 26 27 ◆继邱吉尔出任首相的是谁? 28 误:Who succeeded after Churchill as Prime Minister? 29 正:Who succeeded Churchill as Prime Minister? 30 析:succeed 表示“接替(某人)”,是及物动词,其后无需用介词after。 31 但它若表示“继承(职位、头衔、财产等)”,则是不及物的,后跟宾语时带介词to(此时也不32 用after), 33 如:She succeeded to the mayoralty.(她继任市长职位), 34 比较:She succeeded him to the mayoralty.(她接替他担任市长)。 35 36

(完整版)the的用法

定冠词the的用法: 定冠词the与指示代词this ,that同源,有“那(这)个”的意思,但较弱,可以和一个名词连用,来表示某个或某些特定的人或东西. (1)特指双方都明白的人或物 Take the medicine.把药吃了. (2)上文提到过的人或事 He bought a house.他买了幢房子. I've been to the house.我去过那幢房子. (3)指世界上独一无二的事物 the sun ,the sky ,the moon, the earth (4)单数名词连用表示一类事物 the dollar 美元 the fox 狐狸 或与形容词或分词连用,表示一类人 the rich 富人 the living 生者 (5)用在序数词和形容词最高级,及形容词等前面 Where do you live?你住在哪? I live on the second floor.我住在二楼. That's the very thing I've been looking for.那正是我要找的东西. (6)与复数名词连用,指整个群体 They are the teachers of this school.(指全体教师) They are teachers of this school.(指部分教师) (7)表示所有,相当于物主代词,用在表示身体部位的名词前 She caught me by the arm.她抓住了我的手臂. (8)用在某些有普通名词构成的国家名称,机关团体,阶级等专有名词前 the People's Republic of China 中华人民共和国 the United States 美国 (9)用在表示乐器的名词前 She plays the piano.她会弹钢琴. (10)用在姓氏的复数名词之前,表示一家人 the Greens 格林一家人(或格林夫妇) (11)用在惯用语中 in the day, in the morning... the day before yesterday, the next morning... in the sky... in the dark... in the end... on the whole, by the way...

(完整版)副词与形容词的用法

副词和形容词 一、形容词的用法 1.形容词修饰名词,并且放在名词的前面,这时形容词在句子中作定语 例如: a beautiful lady、a tall man、a big house A beautiful lady is standing in front of a tall man. 2.形容词放在be动词的后面,这时形容词在句子中作表语/主语补足语。 例如:The lady is tall. (tall在句子中作表语,说明lady是怎么样的) The beautiful lady is tall. (beautiful在句子中作定语,tall作表语) The beautiful lady is tall and slim. 3.形容词放在连系动词(become成为、seem看起来、taste尝起来、look看起来、smell 闻起来、feel摸起来/感觉、turn变成,等等)后面,在句子中作表语/主语补足语。 例如:The leaf (叶子) turned yellow.树叶变黄了。 She looks beautiful./ He looks handsome. 她看起来漂亮。/他看来帅气。 The food taste good. 这些食物好吃。 The sweater feels soft. (柔软的;舒服的) He becomes careful. (小心的) 他变得小心了。 The flower smells very good. 花闻起来很香。 Everything seems good. 一切看起来都好。 二、副词的用法 1.副词修饰动词,并且通常放在实义动词后面,这是副词在句子中作方式状语。 例如:The man runs fast. (fast修饰runs这个动作) She jumps high. (high修饰jump这个动作) He finished his homework quickly. (quickly修饰finished这个动作) 2.副词修饰形容词,并且通常放在形容词的前面 例如:He becomes very handsome. She looks very beautiful. The lady is very tall and slim. 在“副词+形容词”这样的结构中,中心词是形容词,副词只是为了说明程度大小 即:very handsome的中心词是handsome 3.副词前面也可以加副词,例如上面的句子都可以改写成: The man runs very fast. She jumps very high. He finished his homework very quickly. very本身是副词,意思是“非常,很”,所以后面也可以跟副词或者形容词。 三、填形容词还是副词? 动词后面一般都跟副词,但不是所有动词后面都跟副词,实义动词后面跟副词,连系动词后面跟形容词。 例如:She sings beautifully. (sing是实义动词,beautiful用来说明唱得如何) Tom draws well.(draw是实义动词,well用来说明画得如何) My teacher is young and tall.(is是系动词,后面跟形容词) She looks sad. (look是连系动词,后面跟形容词)

success用法专项

例题: He took a second driving test and finally ________. A. succeeded in passing it B. succeeded in it C. succeeded to through D. succeeded to pass it. succeed in + doing sth = 成功做成某事 succeed to ...: 继承(王位等) succeed sb: 继承 succeed with sth:在某方面获得成功 succeed,success,successful的区别和用法 一、succeed的用法 ◆他的计划成功了。 误:His plan was succeeded. 正:His plan succeeded. 析:succeed 表示“成功”,是不及物动词,因此不能用于被动语态。 ◆他终于把那个问题解决了。 误:At last he succeeded to solve the problem. 正:At last he succeeded in solving the problem. 析:表示做某事做成功了,succeed 后通常不接不定式,而接 in doing sth。 又如:He succeeded in getting the job.(他谋到了那份工作), She succeeded in (passing) the exam.(她考试及格了)。 注:有时此结构可用作反语,如:I tried to clean the watch, but only succeeded in breaking it. (我想把表弄干净,结果却弄坏了)。 ◆继邱吉尔出任首相的是谁 误:Who succeeded after Churchill as Prime Minister

重要单词用法

bid/b?d/CET4 TEM4( bidding, bids ) 1. N-COUNT A bid for something or a bid to do something is an attempt to obtain it or do it. 努力尝试[journalism]例: ...Sydney's successful bid for the 2000 Olympic Games. …悉尼对2000年奥林匹克运动会成功的申办。 2. N-COUNT A bid is an offer to pay a particular amount of money for something that is being sold. 出价 例: Hanson made an agreed takeover bid of $351 million. 汉森按约定出价3.51亿美元进行收购。 3. V-T/V-I If you bid for something or bid to do something, you try to obtain it or do it. 力求获得; 努力争取 例: Singapore Airlines is rumoured to be bidding for a management contract to run both airports. 据传,新加坡航空公司正在努力争取这两个机场的管理合约。 4. V-I If you bid for something that is being sold, you offer to pay a particular amount of money for it. 出价 例: She wanted to bid for it. 她想出价买下它。 例: The bank announced its intention to bid. 银行宣布了其投标意向。 blunder/?bl?nd?/CET6 TEM8( blundering, blundered, blunders ) 1. N-COUNT A blunder is a stupid or careless mistake. 愚蠢错误 例: I think he made a tactical blunder by announcing it so far ahead of time. 我认为他如此提早宣布消息是犯了战术上的错误。 2. V-I If you blunder, you make a stupid or careless mistake. 犯愚蠢错误 例: No doubt I had blundered again. 不用说我又犯了个蠢错。 3. V-I If you blunder into a dangerous or difficult situation, you get involved in it by mistake. 误入(危险境地或困境) 例: People wanted to know how they had blundered into war, and how to avoid it in the future. 人们想弄清他们怎么会错误地卷入战争,将来如何才能避免这样的事。 4. V-I If you blunder somewhere, you move there in a clumsy and careless way. 跌跌撞撞地走 例: He had blundered into the table, upsetting the flowers. 他撞上了桌子,打翻了花。 cater/?ke?t?/CET6 TEM4( catering, catered, caters )

succeed用法

succeed v. 成功★★★ If you want to succeed, always force yourself to do more. 如果你想要成功,总是要强迫自己多做一点。 Did you succeed in the interview (常与in连用,表示“成功;达到;完成”) 你面试成功了吗 特别奉献: 全新单词记忆法之五:单词家族一网打尽! 英语中很多单词都有整个家族,名词、动词、形容词、副词,加上不同的后缀、前缀,就有不同的词性变化、意思变化。要想彻底掌握一个单词,我们要做的就是:整个家族一锅端!家族内所有单词全消灭! 象succeed success successful unsuccessful这样一个“伟大”的家族,动词、名词、形容词,还有个副词successfully,一次就全部攻克! 21世纪超级拿手好戏: succeed家族一网打尽 说出succeed这个词的感觉一定很棒,因为人人都想成功!下面再送大家几个句子: ☆ If you try hard, you will succeed. 如果你努力, 就会成功。

☆ He succeeded in the examination. 他考试及格了。 ☆ His business has succeeded, and is making a lot of money. 他的生意很成功,赚了很多钱。 “succeed”家族成员还有: success n. 成功;胜利;成功之人;成功的事★★★★ ☆ Her success as a popular singer was short-lived. 作为流行歌曲歌手她取得的成功只是昙花一现。 ☆ If you want to be a success in business you must be aggressive. 如果你想在生意上获得成功,你必须有点闯劲。 ☆ Never be satisfied with just a little success. 不要有一点成绩就满足。 ☆ Failure is the mother of success. 失败是成功之母。 ☆ He was not a success as a governor, but he became a great president. 就做州长而言, 他不是一个成功者,但他成了一个伟大的总统。 ☆ The conference was a success.

常用词含义用法

有道词典·看天下 带你用另一种方式看世界! ?关注我 o?RSS订阅 ?RSS Feed ?订阅到 QQ邮箱 ?订阅到有道阅读 ?订阅到鲜果 ?订阅到抓虾 ?订阅到 iGoogle oǔ新浪微博 oǖ腾讯微博 首页实用口语口语达人背单词听说练习双语阅读微英语单词卡片 学了很多年英语,有时还是搞不清 but 的正确用法 来源|知乎日报作者|猪小宝

有时候,我对自己的写作能力有一种愚蠢的自信。不过在知乎上,尤其是知乎日报上,我发现好些在我的回答底下评论的人,可能读不懂我的意思。我猜,有可能是他们的阅读能力有问题,也有可能是我的写作能力有问题。 这学期在学习学术英语写作,正好在这里做点学习笔记,提高一下自己的写作能力。以前觉得自己托福写作满分,没什么问题,上了这课才知道,差得太远了。 跟汉语相比,我个人觉得,英语更为简洁、更为逻辑化,规则掌握好,就能比较容易的写出不错的文章。当然,写作的种类有很多种,诗歌、小说、新闻、法律文书,各有各的特点,各有各的规则。我在这里说的只适用于学术写作,更确切的说只适用于理工科的学术写作。 永远不要用 And、But、So 作为句子的开头 这一类词的学名叫 coordinating conjunctions,俗称叫 FANBOYS,因为一共就七个:for, and, nor, but, or, yet, and so。这一类词不能用在句子的开头,不能首字母大写作为句子的第一个单词。如果要用类似的词作为句子的开头,需要用 subordinating conjunctions 代替,比如 therefore, however 等。 错误用法:John is rich. But he is unhappy. 正确用法:John is rich, but he is unhappy. 正确用法:John is rich. However, he is unhappy. 表示因果关系时 because 的用法 错误用法:I bought a sandwich. Because I was hungry. 正确用法:I bought a sandwich because I was hungry. 正确用法:Because I was hungry, I bought a sandwich. 这里的例句很简单,即使是第一个错误用法,读者也能很容易猜出意思。但在实际的学术写作中,原因和结果可能都是非常复杂的句子,这时候就需要 because 的正确用法来告诉读者到底是怎样的因果关系。 比如,AAA because BBB. CCC. 这是说 BBB 是 AAA 的原因。再比如,AAA. Because BBB, CCC. 这是说 BBB 导致了 CCC。 如果是用的第一种错误用法,AAA. Because BBB. CCC. 读者就一头雾水了,到底 BBB 是 AAA 的原因呢,还是 CCC 的原因呢。 尽量做到一句话以旧信息开头,新信息结束 所谓的旧信息,指的是上文已经提及的信息,或者读者早已知道的信息;所谓的新信息,指的是这个句子的重点,这个句子的目的就是为了提供给读者这个信息,你认为应该着重强调的信息。

success的用法总结

success的用法总结 success可作为可数名词和不可数名词使用,那么你分得清它在什么情况下用作可数名词和不可数名词吗? 释义 success英[s?k?ses] 美[s?k?ses] n. 成功,成就;胜利;大获成功的人或事物 用法 success做名词时,既做可数名词也做不可数名词。 当其作可数名词时,意思为成功,胜利,发财,成名。 具体用法如下:success in sth/doing sth,have much(little etc.) success in sth , the key to success ,meet with little success 。 做不可数名词时,意思为,成功的人或事,make a success of sth。 succeed为动词,有四种意思: 1,达到目的,实现目标,办到,做成,为不及物动词,具体用法:succeed in doing sth 2.成功,有成就,有作为,为不及物动词用法:succeed in sth succeed as sth 3.接替,继任,随后出现,为及物动词如:Obama succeeded Bush as president.(奥巴马接任布什成为总统)。 4.继承,为不及物动词:如succeed to sth.如:She succeeded

to the throne.她继承了王位。 successful为形容词,有两个意思: 1.达到目的,有成效的。用法:be successful in sth/doing sth ,be successful at sth/doing sth 2.获得成功的,有成就的。如:a successful actor 有成就的演员。 短语 success in 在…方面成功 great success 巨大成功;非常成功 with success 成功地 success rate 成功率;接通率 without success 没有成功 secret of success n. 成功的秘诀 achieve success 取得成功;获得成功 success and failure 成功与失败 way to success 成功之路 road to success 成功之路;成功者之路 complete success 完全成功;彻底的胜利 make a success of 把…做得十分成功,在…上取得成功(或做出成绩) economic success 经济上的成功 success story n. 一个人的成名史

英语中各类词的用法

英语中各种词的用法 介词是一种用来表示词与词, 词与句之间的关系的词。在句中不能单独作句字成分。介词后面一般有名词代词或相当于名词的其他词类,短语或从句作它的宾语。介词和它的宾语构成介词词组,在句中作主语, 状语,表语,补语或介词宾语。例如: Most of the students went to the classroom. 大部分学生去了教室。 We play basketball on the sports ground. 我们在操场上打蓝球。 介词常与动词,形容词,名词一起构成固定搭配。 belong to 属于rely on 依靠 talk to 同...谈话be afraid of 害怕 be strict with对...严格 介词一般放在名词之前。但它后面的介词宾语是疑问代词,疑问副词或者关系代词时,这些词提到了前面而只剩下介词在后了。 Where do you come from? 你是哪儿人? Who are you talking to? 你在跟谁谈话呢? What do you study for? 你为了什么而学习? 介词在英语词汇中所占比例很小,但它们的用法却非常灵活,复杂。以下是一个例子: about 关于,附近,大约,周围,随身. I have bought a book about Shakespearean. 我买了一本有关莎士比亚的书。 1. WITH (1)v+with (a) v+with begin, mix, agree, deal, fight, meet, play, quarrel, do, fool, reason, correspond, comply, settle. (b) v + sth (sb) + with + sth (sb) compare, provide, supply, feed, replace, combine, equip, furnish,

形容词的用法

形容词的用法 【真题再现】 1. — Have you watched A Bite of China recently? (2014 山西) — Of course. I do believe Chinese food is ______ in the world. A. the most delicious B. more delicious C. delicious 2. John is getting very thin. He doesn't eat _______ food. (2014 沈阳) A. many B. enough C. few D. little 3. You have to be ______ and wait until I finish my work. (2014江西) A. patient B. strict C. honest D. active 4. Overseas experience may help make our life . So why not try to study abroad?(2014 东营) A. usual B. useful C. successful D. traditional 5. — Tom, are you boy in your class?(2014 滨州) — No, but John is. I’m shorter than him. A. the tallest B. the shortest C. the youngest D. the oldest 6. Mr. Black used to be busy. But now he's retired and , so he has plenty of time to exer-cise. (2014 连云港) A. hard B. calm C. free D. nervous 7. — Do you enjoy Han Lei's songs?(2014 南京) — Yes. He is the winner of I'm Singer II. I can't think of anyone with a ______ voice. A. better B. best C. more D. most 8. Of the two shirts, I’d like to choose the ________ one to save some money for a cap.(2014 杭 州) A. cheapest B. cheaper C. more expensive D. most expensive 9. How ________ Cindy grows! She's almost as tall as her mother now.(2014 河北) A. cute B. strong C. fast D. straight 10. —I’ll not be Jack’s friend any more. (2014 荆州) —Don’t be angry. He’s just so , but in fact he’s good to us, you know. A. helpful B. direct C. polite D. brave 11. —How do you like this house? (2015 兰州) —_______ It’s everything I’ve been looking for. A. Terrible! B. Perfect! C. Awful! D. Delicious! 12. Mom, I’m very _________ for all your love.(2015 河北) A. thankful B. careful C. useful D. helpful 13. A person who is __________ does not tell lies or cheat people. (2015 温州) A. careless B. stupid C. honest D. humorous 14. —The scarves are all beautiful. I can’t decide which one to choose. (2015 南昌) —Oh, look at this red one. I think it’s______. A. beautiful B. more beautiful C. the most beautiful D. less beautiful 15. Lisa was still very weak when she left hospital.But after a week’s rest, she felt much ______ and went back to school.(2015 南京) A.good B.better C.bad D.worse

best的详细用法

1.最好的 best ?He won the best actor award. 他获得最佳男演员奖。 ?What’s the best way to cook this fish? 这鱼怎么烧最好? ?The best thing to do is to stop worrying. 最好的办法就是不要再担心。?Our pilots are given the best possible training. 我们的飞行员接受最好的训练。 ?We use only the very best ingredients. 我们只用最好的原料。 it’s best to do sth?It’s best to go later in the season. 最好是当季晚些时候去。 easily the best/by far the best (=much better than anything else)绝对是最好的?John’s idea is by far the best option. 约翰的主意绝对是最好的选择。 3.best dress/shoes/clothes etc最好的礼服/鞋子/衣服等〔指留待特殊场合穿戴的衣物等〕?I put on my best suit for the wedding. 我穿上最好的一套衣服参加婚礼。 best 2 1.最好地?It works best if you let it warm up first. 如果先把它预热一下,使用效果最佳。 2.最,极SYN MOST ?You know him best – you should ask him. 你最熟悉他,应该你去问他。 3.as best you can尽最大努力,竭力?I’ll try and fix it as best I can. 我会尽力把它修好。 4.had best应该,最好?We’d best be getting back. 我们最好回去。 best 3 1.the best最佳;至上;至善?We all want the best for our children. 我们都希望给孩子最好的东西。 the person or thing that is better than any other最好之人;最佳之物?She’s the best of the new young writers. 她是新一代年轻作家中最优秀的。 2.do your best尽力而为;尽最大努力?As long as you do your best, we’ll be happy. 只要你尽力而为,我们就满意了。 do your best to do sth?She did her best to make him comfortable. 她尽力让他感到舒服。 3.at best充其量;至多?The campaign was at best only partially successful. 这场宣传活动充其量只是部分成功。?The technique is at best ineffective and at worst dangerous. 这个方法说轻一点是无效,说重一点是危险。 4.to the best of your knowledge/belief/ability etc就某人所知/在某人看来/尽某人所能等?I’m sure he’ll do the work to the best of his ability. 我相信他会尽己所能做好这项工作。 5.the best of sth最好的某事物?We wish him the best of luck with this venture. 我们祝愿他在这个风险项目上顺顺利利。 6.with the best of intentions/for the best of reasons出于好心,出于好意?I’m sure he went there with the best of intentions. 我相信他去那里是出于好心。 7.the best of both worlds两全其美;各取其长

相关主题
相关文档
最新文档