市场营销外文文献

市场营销外文文献
市场营销外文文献

JCPS-00396; No. of pages: 10; 4C:

Available online at https://www.360docs.net/doc/c110303719.html,

ScienceDirect

Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx–xxx

Research Article

Sensory marketing, embodiment, and grounded cognition: A review

and introduction

Aradhna Krishna a, Norbert Schwarz b , c ,?

a

Ross School of Business, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, USA

b Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USA

c Marshall School of Business, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USA

Received 22 December 2013; accepted 23 December 2013

Abstract

There has been a recent swell of interest in marketing as well as psychology pertaining to the role of sensory experiences in judgment and decision making. Within marketing, the ?eld of sensory marketing has developed which explores the role of the senses in consumer behavior. In

psychology, the dominant computer metaphor of information processing has been challenged by researchers demonstrating various manners in

which mental activity is grounded in sensory experience. These ?ndings are arduous to explain using the amodal model of the human mind. In this

introduction, we ?rst delineate key assumptions of the information processing paradigm and then discuss some of the key conceptual challenges posed by the research generally appearing under the titles of embodiment, grounded cognition, or sensory marketing. We then address the use of bodily feelings as a source of information; next, we turn to the role of context sensitive perception, imagery, and simulation in consumer behavior,

and ?nally discuss the role of metaphors. Through this discourse, we note the contributions to the present special issue as applicable.

? 2013 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction

In recent years, the role of sensory experiences in judgment

and decision making has seen a surge of interest in marketing as well as psychology. In marketing, scattered research on the role

of the senses in consumer behavior has been brought together under the rubric of sensory marketing, that is, “marketing that engages the consumers' senses and affects their perception, judgment, and behavior”(Krishna, 2012, p. 332; for reviews, see Krishna, 2012, 2013, and the contributions in Krishna, 2010). In psychology, researchers challenged the dominant computer metaphor of information processing by highlighting ways in which mental activity is grounded in sensory experience (for reviews, see Barsalou, 2008; Meier, Schnall, Schwarz, & Bargh, 2012; Niedenthal, Barsalou, Winkielman, Krauth-Gruber,

& Ric, 2005). The rapidly accumulating empirical findings are ?Corresponding author at: Dept of Psychology, University of Southern often surprising and difficult to account for within the amodal model of the human mind that has dominated psychology and consumer research since the cognitive revolution of the 1970s. The findings also amplify earlier challenges to models of rational choice —anybody who was dismayed by the observation that different ways of presenting a choice alternative can affect choice (for reviews, see the contributions in Lichtenstein & Slovic, 2006) will be even more dismayed by the extent to which incidental exposure to sensory experiences in one domain can affect choice in another, substantively unrelated domain (for a review, see Lee & Schwarz, 2014).

As the history of science illustrates, at early stages of a research program the focus is often on identifying challenges to dominant paradigms in the hope that they will capture a field's attention (Root-Bernstein, 1989). At that stage, documenting novel phenomena that are difficult to account for by the dominant paradigm takes precedence over understanding the details of how the new phenomena operate. Consistent with this regularity, early

2013 Society for Consumer Psychology. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: Krishna, A., & Schwarz, N., Sensory marketing, embodiment, and grounded cognition: A review and introduction, Journal of Consumer

2 A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx–xxx

judgment and decision making, whereas distinctions between different theoretical approaches to embodiment received less attention (for an early discussion, see Anderson, 2008). More recently, researchers began testing competing theories of the underlying processes; history suggests that this will be the focus

of future research in this area. At present, however, there is wide consensus that we cannot understand human cognition without taking into account that humans interact with the world through their senses and do their thinking within a body; but there is much less consensus about what this implies and how it is best conceptualized. Moreover, the different emerging theoretical perspectives are not necessarily mutually exclusive and different processes may contribute to observed effects under different conditions.

Our introduction to this special issue of the Journal of Consumer Psychology summarizes these developments, notes different theoretical perspectives, and places the contributions

to the special issue in their context. We begin by revisiting key assumptions of the information processing paradigm that has dominated consumer psychology for the last four decades and note some of the conceptual challenges posed by recent research commonly presented under the headings of embodi- ment, grounded cognition, or sensory marketing. As this research moves beyond initial attention-grabbing illustrations, researchers are increasingly identifying limiting conditions and competing pathways, a step that is central to understand-

ing the phenomena and the conditions under which they can

(or cannot) be reliably replicated. While the often counterin- tuitive and amusing nature of some of the iconic findings adds

to their popular interest value, it also distracts casual observers (and reviewers) from the fact that the findings pose serious challenges to how we think about the human mind. Next, we note these conceptual challenges.

Information processing, with and without a body:

a historical perspective

In the 1970s, the development of general models of information processing provided an integrative framework for the conceptualization of human memory, judgment, and decision making (for authoritative contemporary treatments, see Lachman, Lachman, & Butterfield, 1979; Wyer, 1974). In a nutshell, the approach assumed that people acquire information through their senses and that this information is translated into a common code that is independent of the modality in which the information was initially acquired. These amodal representa- tions were assumed to be the building blocks of knowledge (semantic memory), on which the mind operates with general procedures that are also independent of the initial modality. Once stored, knowledge representations were assumed to be context independent, although context could influence how easily a stored representation could be accessed. These assumptions were consistent with a computer metaphor that emphasized encoding, storage, and retrieval operations and took it for granted that all operations require a common format, paralleling the operational requirements of a computer. Their break with centuries of earlier theorizing that took it for granted that visual, olfactory or haptic inputs were stored in a form

that reflected the modality of their acquisition. In fact, having

a controversial discussion about whether imagery involves anything like an image (e.g., Paivio, 1971; Pylyshyn, 1973)

would have been unimagineable prior to the information processing paradigm (and the controversy has been settled in favor of modal representations; see Kosslyn, Thompson, & Ganis, 2006).

While the information processing paradigm facilitated enormous progress in psychological research, it also came

with its own blind spots (for a discussion, see Schwarz, 2000).

Most notably, it neglected phenomena that were not easily conceptualized within a computer metaphor, including the role

of moods, emotions, and other subjective experiences as well as the role of motivation, which was reduced to processing goals. Given their subject matter, these limitations were particularly apparent to social psychologists, who integrated the role of feelings and “hot”motivational processes in the social cognition variants of information processing models, paying tribute to

social psychology's long tradition of attending to visceral aspects

of social perception and behavior (for reviews, see Schwarz & Clore, 2007; Strack & Deutsch, 2007). For the most part, however, social cognition theories did not fundamentally challenge the assumption of amodal representations. Instead, feelings and bodily states were themselves assumed to be stored

in memory in an amodal form, for example, as nodes in a semantic network (e.g., Bower, 1981), although they provided “online”information that competed with the amodally represent- ed “offline”information stored in memory (e.g., Schwarz, 1990). Some current embodiment research is compatible with this tradition and addresses how concurrent bodily experiences serve as a source of information in judgment and choice; these processes can be conceptualized within feelings-as-information theory (Schwarz, 2012) and we address this work in the section

on bodily feelings as information.

Going beyond these extensions of the information process-

ing paradigm, the assumption of a largely decontextualized and amodal mind has been questioned more radically by several streams of research that converge in support of an emerging metatheoretical narrative that emphasizes the situated, experien- tial, and embodied nature of human cognition. Following early insights of William James (1890), situated cognition research assumes that thinking is for doing and that any mind worth having needs to be geared towards facilitating action in ways that are sensitive to the requirement of one's current situation (for a review, see Smith & Semin, 2004). This perspective turns contextual influences on judgment and choice from deplorable “context dependency”into laudable “context sensitivity”, with far reaching consequences for the conceptualization of preferences, attitudes, and related concepts (for a discussion, see Schwarz, 2007). Because people experience the world through their senses, sensory information and the accompanying subjective experi- ences play a key role in human action and cognition, even in domains that seem abstract and far removed from immediate sensory inputs. Some suggest that this reflects that humans

A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx –xxx

3

primarily to perceptual and motoric processing ” (Wilson, 2002, p. 625). The later development of higher mental processes presumably took advantage of these evolutionarily older programs and reused them for new purposes (Anderson, 2010). From this perspective, what psychologists traditionally referred to as “higher mental processes ” is grounded in bodily experience because it reuses phylogenetically older neural circuitry (for a review of supporting evidence, see Anderson, 2010). Others emphasize ontogenetic rather than phylogenetic learning and suggest that early sensory experience with the physical and social world (e.g., feeling warm and safe in the presence of a caregiver)

structures later thinking about abstract concepts (e.g., security), resulting in associations between sensory experience and abstract

thought across content domains (e.g., Williams, Huang, & Bargh, 2009). Independent of specific assumptions about their origin, links

between sensory experience and abstract concepts are often reflected in widely shared metaphors (for a review, see Landau, Meier, & Keefer, 2010). For example, a relationship may be

described as “cold ” or “warm ”— and, indeed, experiencing

physical warmth through touching a warm object increases the

perception of social warmth in others (e.g., Williams & Bargh, 2008), much as the social experience of a “cold shoulder ” influences perceptions of the physical temperature of the room

(e.g., Zhong & Leonardelli, 2008). A large part of current embodiment research in consumer psychology addresses such

metaphorical influences; we discuss key findings and concep- tual controversies in the section on metaphors.

Finally, the most radical approach to grounded cognition conceptualizes all mental acts as acts of modality specific sensory

simulation (e.g., Barsalou, 1999; Barsalou, Niedenthal, Barbey, & Ruppert, 2003). From this perspective, “as an experience occurs (e.g., easing into a chair), the brain captures states across

the modalities and integrates them with a multimodal represen-

tation stored in memory (e.g., how a chair looks and feels, the action of sitting, introspections of comfort and relaxation)” (Barsalou, 2008, p. 618). Such multimodal representations

can be activated through any of the participating modalities. More important, thinking about a chair is assumed to involve a

simulation of previous sensory experience, making simula- tions of earlier experience an essential part of any mental

activity. Supporting this rationale, a large body of neuroim- aging findings shows that supposedly amodal “high level ” processes — such as language comprehension, categoriza- tion, or retrieval — entail activation of the brain areas

involved in “low level ” sensory processing (for a review, see

Barsalou, 2008). This approach is increasingly informing

research in sensory marketing that explores the role of mental simulation in advertising and related issues (e.g., Elder & Krishna, 2012), as reviewed below. The next three sections elaborate on these core themes. We first address the use of bodily feelings as a source of information, which is most compatible with traditional social cognition models; next, we turn to the role of context sensitive

perception, imagery, and simulation in consumer behavior, and finally discuss the role of metaphors. Throughout, we note the

Bodily experience as a source of information The assumption that people attend to their own bodily states and extract information from them has a long tradition in art, philosophy, and psychology. From the heart that beats faster when seeing the loved one to the cold sweat of fear, perceptions of bodily states figure prominently in poems, songs, and

popular images. Experimental studies confirm that bodily states inform human judgment and that people use this information as they use any other information (for reviews, see Herbert &

Pollatos, 2012; Schwarz & Clore, 2007). Consistent with the logic of feelings-as-information theory (for a recent review, see

Schwarz, 2012), the impact of bodily states depends on their perceived informational value; it is attenuated or eliminated when the experience is attributed to an unrelated source, and

enhanced when it is experienced despite contrary influences. A fast beating heart indeed increases the attractiveness of pin-up photos, unless one realizes that the elevated heart beat may be

the result of having climbed several flights of stairs (Zillman, 1978). Similarly, physiological symptoms of fear increase the impact of a fear-arousing movie, in particular when they are experienced despite having taken an allegedly tranquilizing

pill; yet their influence is eliminated when the symptoms can be

attributed to an allegedly arousing pill (Schwarz, Servay, & Kumpf, 1985).

Facilitating or impairing a person's bodily response to a stimulus also facilitates or impairs the person's subjective

experience of the stimulus. As Strack, Martin, and Stepper (1988) showed by varying the way in which participants held a

pen in their mouths, a cartoon is perceived as funnier when the viewer's face can respond with a smile than when pursed lips impair a smile, highlighting that facial muscle feedback plays a

crucial role in subjective experience and judgment. In recent years, many consumers voluntarily exposed themselves to a treatment that impairs facial muscle response, namely botox injections. The unanticipated side-effects range from impaired

affective experience (Davis, Senghas, Brandt, & Ochsner, 2010) to an impaired understanding of others' emotional expressions

(Neil & Chartrand, 2011) and impaired processing of emotion words (Havas, Glenberg, Gutowski, Lucarelli, & Davidson,

2010). The latter findings show that understanding others' emotional expression, or merely comprehending emotionally charged words, involves activity of the facial muscles that are part of one's own emotional expression. This is consistent with the

assumption that all mental processes involve sensory simulation (Barsalou, 1999) and draws attention to the role of the body in many basic psychological functions that are usually conceptual- ized in amodal ways. As the body's role in basic psychological processes receives more attention, we will increasingly encounter evidence that familiar psychological phenomena have previously unknown bodily underpinnings. As an example, consider the mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968), that is, the observation that

we like things more, the more often we are exposed to them. This is the case because repeated exposure facilitates easy

processing, which is experienced as pleasant; accordingly, any

4 A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx–xxx

liking (for reviews, see Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004; Schwarz, 2004). In an important series of studies, Topolinski and his colleagues identified the ease of motor processes as a crucial component of many fluency experiences. During reading, spontaneous subvocalization becomes easier with repetition —and impairing subvocalization by having perceivers chew gum eliminates the otherwise observed mere exposure effect, which is not the case for (equally distracting) motor tasks that do not impair subvocalization (Topolinski & Strack, 2009). Similarly, exercises that facilitate the eye movements involved in

a visual perception task enhance later liking of matching visual stimuli (Topolinski, 2010). Taking these observations to consumer behavior, Topolinski et al. (in this issue)show that chewing popcorn in the cinema undermines the effectiveness of advertisements by interfering with subvocalization, thus reducing the fluency of motor processes that underlie language-based mere exposure effects.

Topolinski and colleagues' finding illustrates how a better understanding of the bodily underpinnings of familiar phenom- ena can identify novel limiting conditions, with important theoretical and applied implications (Topolinski, 2012). Looking ahead, we assume that future embodiment research will identify bodily contributions to many other well-established psycholog- ical phenomena. In some cases, this will require a thorough reconceptualization of the phenomenon itself; in other cases, it will deepen our understanding of its component processes by adding a bodily level of analysis.

Finally, just as cognition is impacted by the senses, sensory perception too may be affected by externally provided informa- tion. Elder and Krishna (2010) showed participants text advertisements for foods that either encompassed a single sense or multiple senses; then, they had the subjects taste the food. They found that taste perception was affected by the externally provided information so that the food tasted better when the information encompassed several senses. They explain their results as the advertisement impacting the sensory thoughts that subjects generate which subsequently affects taste perception. While the recent research focus has been on showing the effect of the senses on cognition, little research attention has been devoted to the effect of information on sensory perception. There is much scope for additional research exploring the effect of information on sensory perception.

Sensory experience, mental simulation, and

stimulus attributes

People perceive the world through their senses. But the representations they construct from sensory inputs are not necessarily valid reflections of stimulus properties, a fact that gave rise to an enduring interest in illusions of sensory perception from early philosophy to current neuroscience. Much research in consumer behavior and psychology contributed to documenting such (visual) illusions (e.g., Krishna & Raghubir, 1997; Raghubir

& Krishna, 1996, 1999; Wansink & Van Ittersum, 2003; for a review see Krishna, 2007), showing how they may be impacted Zhang, 2008) or ameliorated by the addition of another sensory input (Krishna, 2006).

Extending the work on sensory illusions, recent research

has revisited the veridicality of perception from the perspective

of goal directed action: If the purpose of cognition is to enable adaptive action, are action relevant aspects of sensory perception less likely to be distorted in maladaptive ways? Conversely, do our goals influence how we “see”the external world? If so, are the distortions adaptive? The answer is

almost certainly a resounding, “It depends!”Nevertheless, the accumulating evidence supports a more favorable perspective

on the role of distortions than most researchers would have assumed a decade ago.

For example, some findings suggest that visual illusions

(e.g., illusions of size) exert more influence on choice and

verbal reports (Which figure is larger?) than on bodily actions, such as the grip strength with which people try to grasp the figure (e.g., Aglioti, DeSouza, & Goodale, 1995). However, the conditions under which this is the case, and how it is properly assessed, are subject of some controversy (Milner & Dyde, 2003; Smeets & Brenner, 2006). Other findings show that perception

is highly sensitive to the perceiver's goals and bodily states and “informs people about opportunities for action and their associated costs”(Proffitt, 2006, p. 110). For example, hills

seem steeper when we wear a heavy backpack (e.g., Proffitt, Bhalla, Gossweiler, & Midgett, 1995), but their slope flattens

when we bring some friends along for social support

(e.g., Schnall, Harber, Stefanucci, & Proffitt, 2008). Such

findings reflect that perception stands in the service of action

and takes the perceiver's resources into account. This “economy-of-action”approach to perception (Proffitt, 2006) sheds new light on classic issues of motivated biases that have preoccupied psychologists since the “New Look”of the 1950's (Bruner, 1957). Cornil, Ordabayeva, Kaiser, Weber, and

Chandon (in this issue) contribute to current analyses of goal sensitive perception by exploring the role of attitude ambiva- lence. On theoretical grounds one might expect that the ambivalence that results from mixed preferences fosters more detailed, and potentially more accurate, assessments. Compatible with this notion, Cornil and colleagues find that ambivalence in

the form of desiring a food that one finds unhealthy can increase perceivers' visual sensitivity to portion size. From an applied perspective, this suggests that framing foods as vices may improve portion size perception among health conscious consumers.

Further highlighting the close link between perception and situated action, numerous studies show that merely perceiving an object elicits simulations of the actions it affords. For example, Tucker and Ellis (1998) showed that seeing the handle of a cup elicits a grasping simulation that is sufficient to interfere with motor performance on a concurrent task. Such spontaneous simulations can even be observed when people merely read a word (e.g., Tucker & Ellis, 2001) —and exposure to “hammer”elicits a different type of grasp (“power”versus “precision”) than exposure to “grape”, reflecting the differential motor activity that would be appropriate in handling these objects. Elder and

A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx–xxx 5

simulations for consumer behavior and advertising. They found higher purchase intentions when the visual orientation of objects

in an advertisement facilitated mental simulation of interacting with the object; for example, a piece of cake seemed more appealing when the fork sitting next to it was oriented toward one's dominant (rather than nondominant) hand, thus facilitating mental simulation of grasping the fork to eat the cake. These beneficial effects of visual orientation were disrupted when participants' dominant hand held another object, which impaired the spontaneous consumption simulation. As in the work of Topolinski et al. (in this issue), incongruent motor activity exerted a profound influence on consumers' preference forma- tion. Such findings, and related evidence bearing on the role of motor simulations in the processing of action language (e.g., Pulvermüller, 2005) and related tasks, raise new questions about the conceptualization of processing fluency (Reber et al., 2004; Schwarz, in press) and call for a closer examination of the sensorimotor underpinnings of metacognitive experience.

We emphasize, however, that not all simulations will exert an influence through the same pathways. Whereas the effects reported by Elder and Krishna (2012) and Topolinski et al. (in this issue) can be plausibly conceptualized as mental motor action affordance or even fluency effects (Reber et al., 2004), the role of mimicry and facial muscle responses in empathy and emotion processing (e.g., Havas et al., 2010; Neil & Chartrand, 2011), for example, is more likely to reflect the role of interoceptive processes in emotion experience; similarly “engagement”through mental simulation can also drive preference as has been shown through dynamic imagery (mental simulation of movement for a static object; Cian, Krishna, & Elder, 2014). Identifying different pathways of simulation effects, and their possible interactions, provides a promising avenue for future research.

Neuropsychological work also indicates that conceptual processing of food information is associated with increased activation of brain areas involved in the processing of tastes and rewards (e.g., Simmons, Martin, & Barsalou, 2005). Indeed, the mental simulation of food consumption is sufficient to elicit satiation, as first shown with explicit simulation instructions (Morewedge, Huh, & Vosgerau, 2010). Going beyond this observation, Larson, Redden, and Elder (in this issue) find that satiation effects can emerge even when mental simulation is automatic rather than deliberate. This work complements research in sensory marketing that addresses how communica- tions (Elder & Krishna, 2012), other senses (e.g., haptics, Biswas, Szocs, Krishna, & Lehmann, 2013; Krishna & Morrin, 2008) and mental simulation (Eelen, Dewitte, & Warlop, 2013; Elder & Krishna, 2012; Shen & Sengupta, 2012) impact taste perception and intention to purchase a food item.

Other sensory research in consumer psychology has produced

a rich body of observations that bear on how sensory attributes of products, advertisements, and retail spaces influence consumers' thoughts, feelings, and decisions (for reviews, see Krishna, 2012, 2013, and the contributions in Krishna, 2010). Much of this work focused on vision. Extending this body of work, Meert, Pandelaere, and Patrick (in this issue) find that adults as well as children prefer glossy objects over matte objects. They argue that surfaces, suggesting that it may be driven by the importance of water in the development of the species. Testing this conjecture, they find that thirst increases consumers' preference for glossiness even in domains unrelated to water, implicating current bodily states in preference formation. Exploring the association of brightness and temperature, Xu and Labroo (in this issue) suggest that ambient brightness may affect perceptions of temperature and may trigger more intense emotional responses. Their findings are compatible with these conjectures. Also addressing brightness, Semin and Palma (in this issue) note a consistent alignment of female gender with light colors and male gender with dark colors. Their experiments show that this alignment affects language processing as well as consumer preferences.

Other consumer research has addressed the haptic properties of consumer goods (Peck & Childers, 2003) and communications (Peck & Wiggins, 2006) and their impact on purchase intention, donation behaviors, and feelings of ownership and endowment (Peck & Shu, 2009; Shu & Peck, 2011). Adding to this stream of research, Brasel and Gips (in this issue) focus on the touch-aspects of computer interfaces. Their findings suggest that the physical interaction with touch screens enhances consumers' sense of psychological ownership and endowment. Complementing their discussion of embodied influences on endowment effects, Florack, Kleber, Busch, and

St?hr (in this issue) note that consumers' sense of ownership can be reduced through physical cleansing. As becomes apparent from these examples, many perceptual attributes are likely to exert (some of) their influence through metaphorical associations. We return to this issue in the next section, which also addresses additional aspects of haptic experience, such as temperature and warmth.

Consumer psychologists studying smell have explored ambient (Mitchell, Kahn, & Knasko, 1995; Morrin & Ratneshwar, 2003)

as well as product smell (Krishna, Lwin, & Morrin, 2010; Lwin, Morrin, & Krishna, 2010; Morrin, Krishna, & Lwin, 2011). Much

of this work has either focused on the effect of ambient smell

on in-store behavior (e.g., Mitchell et al., 1995; Spangenberg, Crowley, & Henderson, 1996) or on showing the strong connection between smell and memory (for reviews of what is currently known about olfaction and the role of scent in marketing see Herz, 2010; Morrin, 2010). For instance, Krishna et al. (2010) show that a distinctly scented object enhances recall for other attributes of the object; this happens both with and without a scent-aid being present at the time of recall, indicating that this is not another form of encoding specificity; furthermore, the memory enhancement effect of scent is larger in the long-term as memory erodes faster in the unscented condition.

Despite the research discussed above, experimental research into smell and audition has remained fairly elusive in consumer psychology, even though scents and sounds are extensively used in marketing (for a review of the role of sounds in the marketplace see Meyers-Levy, Bublitz, & Peracchio, 2009).

One reason for the smaller amount of work on these senses may be the difficulty in controlling olfactory and auditory input, as compared to haptic and taste input. Changing the roughness,

6 A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx–xxx

other haptic aspects constant, is relatively easy. Similarly, changing sweetness or saltiness or foods is straightforward. However, having the same intensity of smell input across participants is a complex and time consuming task; similarly, varying one dimension of sound (e.g., pitch) while controlling for the others poses nontrivial challenges. Hence, the explora-

tion of smell is often restricted to manipulations that require only limited variation of smell intensity or that ask participants to imagine a smell, a task that can affect salivation and desire for food (e.g., Krishna, Morrin, & Sayin, in press).

Metaphors

For many consumer researchers, embodied cognition is probably synonymous with one particular stream of research, namely work that illuminates how sensory experience in one domain can influence cognition, emotion, and behavior in a different domain in ways that follow familiar metaphors. Depending on theoretical perspective, metaphors are either seen as the unique driver of the observed effects (Lakoff, 2008; Lakoff & Johnson, 1999) or as expressions of the multimodal nature of mental representations (Anderson, 2010; Barsalou, 2008; Williams et al., 2009). The current interest in metaphors breaks with a long tradition in philosophy and linguistics that treated metaphors as rhetorical devices best reserved for poets (for a review, see Ortony, 1993). While the underlying processes are subject to some debate (see the contributions in Landau, Robinson, & Meier, 2014), the accumulating evidence makes a compelling case that a metaphor-consistent grounding

of abstract thought in sensory experience can result in pervasive effects of bodily states on targets that are merely metaphorically related (for reviews, see Landau et al., 2010, 2014; Lee & Schwarz, 2014).

One metaphorical link that figures prominently in the present special issue is the association of physical and social warmth, which is abundantly reflected in everyday language (e.g., when we talk about a “warm”person, who is unlikely to be shown a “cold shoulder”). Initial explorations of this metaphor found, for example, that people perceive others as socially warmer after they hold a warm rather than cold cup of coffee (Williams & Bargh, 2008; see also Ijzerman & Semin, 2009) and experience a room as physically colder after having been socially rejected (Zhong & Leonardelli, 2008; see also Izjerman & Semin, 2010). Taking these observations to the consumer domain, Lee, Rotman, and Perkins (2014, this volume) report that consumers perceived the ambient temper- ature to be cooler when eating alone than when eating with a partner; conversely, cool temperatures increased the desire for social consumption settings. Their findings highlight the bidirectional relationship between physical and social warmth (Bargh & Shalev, 2012) and converge with Hong and Sun's (2012) observation that cool temperatures increase consumers' preference for romance movies. As Lee and colleagues note, these findings suggest promising avenues for research into the

role of consumption contexts in the self-regulation of social

Noting that feelings of social closeness are likely to foster conformity, Huang, Zhang, Hui, and Wyer (2014, this volume)

test whether ambient temperature influences conformity in judgment and decision making. In laboratory and field studies they observe higher conformity under warm than cool temperature conditions. Their analysis of underlying process suggests that the opinions of close others exert more influence because they are perceived as more valid —even when the perception of social closeness merely results from a warm room.

Zwebner, Lee, and Goldenberg (in this issue) ask whether

the association of physical and socio-emotional warmth also extends to products. They find that warm ambient temperatures are associated with more positive affective responses and lower perceived distance to the target product, resulting in increased product valuation. This “temperature-premium effect”is robust across several laboratory and field studies and future research may fruitfully address the relative contributions of different affective and metaphorical pathways.

Other contributions to this special issue address sensory variables that have previously received little attention. Xu and Labroo (2014, this volume) suggest that ambient brightness is associated with ambient heat and may foster more extreme affective responses through that association. Their findings are consistent with this prediction across a variety of measures, including consumers' perceptions of aggressiveness and sexiness as well as food preferences. Shalev (in this issue) draws attention to biological links between water supply and energy, which are also reflected in metaphors. She finds that exposure to dryness cues not only increases perceivers' experience of thirst, but

also decreases their own as well as others' perceived level of energy and vitality. More important, perceptions of lower energy, induced through actual thirst or exposure to images of arid land, influenced participants' performance on problem solving tasks.

Whereas the metaphorical link of physical warmth is primarily

to social warmth, other physical dimensions have many different metaphorical associations. A key example is verticality, which is associated with valence (good things are up; e.g., Meier & Robinson, 2004), power (e.g., Schubert, 2005), godliness (e.g., Meier, Hauser, Robinson, Friesen, & Schjeldahl, 2007), rational- ity, and other dimensions of judgment. Which of these diverse meanings of verticality is recruited at a given point in time is likely to depend on its relative accessibility and applicability

to the task at hand, consistent with general principles of the context-sensitive construal of meaning (for a general review, see Schwarz, 2010). Ostinelli, Luna, and Ringberg (in this issue) draw on the association of verticality and valence. Consistent with earlier research, they observe that imagining upward movement has positive evaluative consequences, in their case by boosting feelings of self-worth. Yet high self-worth is not always a beneficial thing (for a review, see Baumeister, Campbell, Krueger, & Vohns, 2003) —while upward movement makes people feel better about themselves, it undermines their motivation for further improvement and hurts their actual task performance. These findings add to the literatures on mental simulation and metaphors

A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx –xxx

7

has shown that washing one's hands after failure eliminates the otherwise observed negative influence of failure on people's self-perceptions but hurts their performance on subsequent tasks by undermining their motivation to exert the required effort (Kaspar, 2013).

Interest in the psychological consequences of physical cleansing was stimulated by an important series of studies by Zhong and Liljenquist (2006), who explored the metaphorical link between physical and moral purity. These links are apparent in language use, from the Psalms' (24:4) notion of “clean hands and a pure heart ” to everyday references to “dirty hands ” or a “dirty mouth ” (Lee & Schwarz, 2010a ). Testing the

behavioral consequences of the physical –moral purity link, Zhong and Liljenquist (2006) found that immoral thoughts

increased the appeal of cleaning products and using those products to clean one's hands eliminated the impact of guilt on

subsequent behavior. Later work found that the power of physical cleansings is not limited to “dirty ” thoughts and behaviors; instead, physical cleansings seem to metaphorically remove diverse traces of the past, from the cognitive dissonance resulting

from difficult choices (Lee & Schwarz, 2010b ) to the impact of previous good or bad luck on risk taking (Xu, Zwick, & Schwarz,

2012) and the influence of failure on subsequent performance (Kaspar, 2013). Such diverse effects, unrelated to moral purity, suggest that physical cleansing can metaphorically remove diverse traces of the past, essentially allowing people to “wipe the slate clean ” (for a review, see Lee & Schwarz, 2011).

Extending the observation of clean-slate effects to another classic

phenomenon of consumer research, Florack et al. (in this issue) report that washing one's hands is also sufficient to attenuate the otherwise robust endowment effect. In their studies, consumers who were endowed with a product were more likely to exchange it for another one after they had cleansed their hands; importantly,

this effect was only observed when participants engaged in an

actual physical cleansing, not when they were merely exposed to

cleaning primes. In the final contribution to this special issue, Marin, Reimann, and Casta ?o (in this issue) show how exposure to

images that carry metaphoric meaning can enhance or impair

perceivers' creativity. Going beyond the demonstration of

novel metaphor effects, they also find that the influence of visual metaphors on creative performance is moderated by perceivers' analogical reasoning skills and requires creative intent.

Whereas consumer psychologists have provided compelling illustrations of the impact of metaphors on consumer behavior, they have mostly remained silent on the broader conceptual debate in psychology and linguistics that centers on the extent

to which metaphors are “merely ” linguistic, embodied, or both

(see, e.g., Barsalou, Santos, Simmons, & Wilson, 2008; Landau et al., 2010). This issue is notoriously difficult to settle and the available evidence indicates that metaphors can exert their

influence through multiple pathways, consistent with the

assumption that mental representations are multimodal. Hence,

it is not surprising that metaphor effects can be observed in response to sensory experiences as well as semantic primes and

Concluding remarks In combination, the contributions to this special issue illustrate the richness of current research at the interface of

sensory marketing, grounded cognition, embodiment, and consumer behavior. Most of the reported findings are difficult to reconcile with standard models of judgment and decision making and reinforce the need to reconsider human cognition as grounded in the sensory experience resulting from goal directed interaction with the world. At the same time, the

challenge to the dominant paradigm is more compelling than

the identification of alternatives and more detailed conjectures about alternative mechanisms, along with focused tests of their

relative merits, would be welcome. We emphasize, however, that testing the breadth and ubiquity of embodiment effects is

itself a valuable contribution to the body of phenomena that need accounting. We were surprised by the extent to which

some reviewers discounted any challenge to standard models of information processing as long as the model is not replaced

with a fully worked out alternative. As Andr é Gide (1925/1973) put it, one cannot discover new lands without consenting to

lose sight of the shore for a while and tolerance for (partial) ambiguity is a prerequisite for creative exploration. Equally surprising was that many of the more than 70 submissions we received made little attempt to address the larger conceptual issues or to even identify the challenge their findings present.

Hopefully, both surprises will become less common as work in this important area progresses. References Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F. X., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions

deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–685. Anderson, M. L. (2008). On the grounds of x ‐grounded cognition. In P. Calvo, & T. Gomila (Eds.), The Elsevier handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach (pp. 423–435). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Anderson, M. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of

the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 245–266.

Bargh, J. A., & Shalev, I. (2012). The substitutability of physical and social warmth in daily life. Emotion, 12, 154–162. Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain

Sciences, 22, 577–660.

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 617–645.

Barsalou, L. W., Niedenthal, P. M., Barbey, A., & Ruppert, J. (2003). Social

embodiment. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 43, 43–92. Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language

and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg,& A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols, embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245–283). New York: Oxford University Press.

Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohns, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happi- ness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest,

4, 1–44.

Biswas, D., Szocs, C., Krishna, A., & Lehmann, D. (2013). Something to chew on:

Effect of food-haptics on mastication and calorie estimation. conditionally accepted at. Journal of Consumer Research. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36,

129–148. Brasel, S. A., & Gips, J. (2014). Tablets, touchscreens, and touchpads: How

varying touch interfaces trigger psychological ownership and endowment.

Journal of Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

8 A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx–xxx

Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123–152. Cian, L., Krishna, A., & Elder, R. (2014). The evoked-dynamism effect: Dynamic imagery from static brand logos. Journal of Marketing Research. Cornil, Y., Ordabayeva, N., Kaiser, U., Weber, B., & Chandon, P. (2014).

The acuity of vice: Attitude ambivalence improves visual sensitivity to

increasing portion sizes. Journal of Consumer Psychology (in this

issue).

Davis, J. I., Senghas, A., Brandt, F., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). The effects of botox injections on emotional experience. Emotion, 10, 433–440. Eelen, J., Dewitte, S., & Warlop, L. (2013). Situated embodied cognition: Monitoring orientation cues affects product evaluation and choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23(4), 424–433.

Elder, R., & Krishna, A. (2010). The effect of advertising copy on sensory thoughts and perceived taste. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(5),

748–756.

Elder, R., & Krishna, A. (2012). The visual depiction effect: Inducing embodied mental simulation that evokes motor responses. Journal of Consumer

Research, 38(6), 988–1003.

Florack, A., Kleber, J., Busch, R., & St?hr, D. (2014). Detaching the ties of ownership: The effects of hand washing on the exchange of endowed

products. Journal of Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

Gide, A. (1925/1973). Les faux-monnayeurs. Paris: Editions Flammarion. Havas, D. A., Glenberg, A. M., Gutowski, K. A., Lucarelli, M. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2010). Cosmetic use of botulinum toxin-A affects processing of

emotional language. Psychological Science, 21, 895–900.

Herbert, B., & Pollatos, O. (2012). The body in the mind. On the relationship between interoception and embodiment. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 692–704.

Herz, R. (2010). The emotional, cognitive and biological basics of olfaction: Implications and considerations for scent marketing. In A. Krishna (Ed.), Sensory marketing: Research on the sensuality of products (pp. 75–86).

New York: Routledge, Psychology Press.

Hong, J., & Sun, Y. (2012). Warm it up with love: The effect of physical coldness on liking of romance movies. Journal of Consumer Research,

39(2), 293–306.

Huang, X. I., Zhang, M., Hui, M. K., & Wyer, R. S. (2014). Warmth and conformity: The effects of ambient temperature on product preferences and financial decisions. Journal of Consumer Psychology.

Ijzerman, H., & Semin, G. R. (2009). The thermometer of social relations.

Psychological Science, 20, 1214–1220.

Izjerman, H., & Semin, G. (2010). Temperature perceptions as a ground for social proximity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 867–873. James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology, Vols. 1–2, . New York: Dover. Kaspar, K. (2013). Washing one's hands after failure enhances optimism but hampers future performance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(1), 69–73.

Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., & Ganis, G. (2006). The case for mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press.

Krishna, A. (2006). The interaction of senses: The Effect of vision and touch on the elongation bias. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(4), 557–566. Krishna, A. (2007). Biases in spatial perception: A review and integrative framework. In M. Wedel,& R. Pieters (Eds.), Visual marketing: From

attention to action. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Krishna, A. (2010). Sensory marketing: Research on the sensuality of products.

New York: Routledge.

Krishna, A. (2012). An integrative review of sensory marketing: Engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 332–351.

Krishna, A. (2013). Customer sense: How the 5 senses influence buying behavior. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Krishna, A., Lwin, M. O., & Morrin, M. (2010). Product scent and memory.

Journal of Consumer Research, 37(1), 57–67.

Krishna, A., & Morrin, M. (2008). Does touch affect taste? The perceptual transfer of product container haptic cues. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 807–818.

Krishna, A., Morrin, M., & Sayin, E. (2013). Drooling over the imagined smell of cookies: A focus on olfactory imagery. Journal of Consumer Research (in press).Krishna, A., & Raghubir, P. (1997). The effect of line configuration on perceived numerosity of dotted lines. Memory and Cognition, 25(4), 492–507. Krishna, A., Zhou, R., & Zhang, S. (2008). The effect of self-construal on spatial judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(2), 337–348. Lachman, R., Lachman, J. T., & Butterfield, E. C. (1979). Cognitive psychology and information processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Lakoff, G. (2008). The neural theory of metaphor. In W. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 17–38). New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenges to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1045–1067.

Landau, M. J., Robinson, M. D., & Meier, B. P. (Eds.). (2014). The power of metaphor: Examining its influence on social life. Washington, DC: APA. Larson, J. S., Redden, J. P., & Elder, R. S. (2014). Satiation from sensory simulation: Evaluating foods decreases enjoyment of similar foods. Journal

of Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

Lee, S. H. M., Rotman, J. D., & Perkins, A. W. (2014). Embodied cognition and social consumption: Self-regulating temperature through social products

and behaviors. Journal of Consumer Psychology.

Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Of dirty hands and dirty mouths: Embodiment of the moral purity metaphor is specific to the motor modality involved in moral transgression. Psychological Science, 21, 1423–1425.

Lee, W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Washing away post-decisional dissonance.

Science, 328, 709.

Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2011). Clean slate effects: The psychological consequences of physical cleansing. Current Directions in Psychological

Science, 20, 307–311.

Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2014). Metaphors in judgment and decision making. In M. J. Landau, M. D. Robinson,& B. P. Meier (Eds.), The power

of metaphor: Examining its influence on social life (pp. 85–108).

Washington, DC: APA.

Lichtenstein, S., & Slovic, P. (Eds.). (2006). The construction of preference.

New York: Cambridge University Press.

Lwin, M., Morrin, M., & Krishna, A. (2010). Exploring the superadditive effects of scent and pictures on verbal recall: An extension of dual coding

theory. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(3), 317–326.

Marin, A., Reimann, M., & Casta?o, R. (2014). Metaphors and creativity: Direct, moderating, and mediating effects. Journal of Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

Meert, K., Pandelaere, M., & Patrick, V. M. (2014). Taking a shine to it: How the preference for glossy stems from an innate need for water. Journal of

Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

Meier, B. P., Hauser, D. J., Robinson, M. D., Friesen, C. K., &

Schjeldahl, K. (2007). What's“up”with God? Vertical space as a

representation of the divine. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 93, 699–710.

Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up:

Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science,

15, 243–247.

Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N., & Bargh, J. (2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 705–716.

Meyers-Levy, J., Bublitz, M. G., & Peracchio, L. A. (2009). The sounds of the marketplace: The role of audition in marketing. In A. Krishna (Ed.), Sensory

marketing: Psychological research for consumers (pp. 137–157). Hove,

UK: Psychology Press.

Milner, D., & Dyde, R. (2003). Why do some perceptual illusions affect visually guided action, when others don't? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7,

10–11.

Mitchell, D. J., Kahn, B. E., & Knasko, S. C. (1995). There's something in the air: Effects of congruent or incongruent ambient odor on consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 22, 229–238.

Morewedge, C. K., Huh, Y. E., & Vosgerau, J. (2010). Thought for food: Imagined consumption reduces actual consumption. Science, 330(6010), 1530–1533. Morrin, M. (2010). Scent marketing: An overview. In A. Krishna (Ed.), Sensory marketing: Research on the sensuality of products (pp. 75–86). New York:

Routledge, Psychology Press.

A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx–xxx 9

Morrin, M., Krishna, A., & Lwin, M. (2011). Retroactive interference from scent cues and the effect on product recall. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(3), 354–361.

Morrin, M., & Ratneshwar, S. (2003). Does it make sense to use scents to enhance brand memory? Journal of Marketing Research, 40, 10–25. Neil, D. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2011). Embodied emotion perception: Amplifying and dampening facial feedback modulates emotion percep-tion accuracy. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2,

673–678.

Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric,

F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion.

Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.

Ortony, A. (1993). Metaphor, language, and thought. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 1–18). New York: Cambridge University Press. Ostinelli, M., Luna, D., & Ringberg, T. (2014). When up bringsyou down: The effects of imagined vertical movements on motivation,

performance, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart& Winston.

Peck, J., & Childers, T. L. (2003). To have and to hold: The influence of haptic information on product judgments. Journal of Marketing, 67, 35–48. Peck, J., & Shu, S. (2009). The effect of mere touch on perceived ownership.

Journal of Consumer Research, 36(3), 434–447.

Peck, J., & Wiggins, J. (2006). It just feels good: Customers' affective response to touch and its Influence on persuasion. Journal of Marketing, 70(4), 56–69.

Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action.

Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.

Proffitt, D. R., Bhalla, M., Gossweiler, R., & Midgett, J. (1995). Perceiving geographical slant. Psychonomic Bulletin& Review, 2, 409–428.

Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Review Neuroscience, 6, 576–582.

Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1973). What the mind's eye tells the mind's brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 1–24.

Raghubir, P., & Krishna, A. (1996). As the crow flies: Bias in consumers' map-based distance judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 23, 26–39. Raghubir, P., & Krishna, A. (1999). Vital dimensions: Antecedents and consequences of biases in volume perceptions. Journal of Marketing

Research, 36(3), 313–326.

Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience?

Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 364–382.

Root-Bernstein, R. S. (1989). Discovering. Inventing and solving problems at the frontiers of scientific knowledge. Cambridge,MA: Harvard University Press.

Schnall, S., Harber, K. D., Stefanucci, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Social support and the perception of geographical slant. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1246–1255.

Schubert, T. W. (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 1–21. Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In E. T. Higgins,& R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.),

Handbook of motivation and cognition. Foundations of social behavior, Vol. 2. (pp. 527–561). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Schwarz, N. (2000). Agenda 2000: Attitudes and social judgment—Warmer, more social, and less conscious. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 149–176.

Schwarz, N. (2004). Meta-cognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 332–348. Schwarz, N. (2007). Attitude construction: Evaluation in context. Social Cognition, 25, 638–656.

Schwarz, N. (2010). Meaning in context: Metacognitive experiences. In B.

Mesquita, L. F. Barrett,& E. R. Smith (Eds.), The mind in context

(pp. 105–125). New York: Guilford.

Schwarz, N. (2012). Feelings-as-information theory. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A.

Kruglanski,& E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social

psychology (pp. 289–308). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Schwarz, N. (2013). Metacognition. In E. Borgida,& J. A. Bargh (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology. Attitudes and social

cognition, Vol. 1, . Washington, DC: APA (in press).

Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal

experiences. In A. Kruglanski,& E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social

psychology. Handbook of basic principles (pp. 385–407) (2nd ed.).

New York: Guilford.

Schwarz, N., Servay, W., & Kumpf, M. (1985). Attribution of arousal as a mediator of the effectiveness of fear-arousing communications. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 74–84.

Semin, R. G., & Palma, T. A. (2014). Why the bride wears white—

Grounding gender with brightness. Journal of Consumer Psychology (in

this issue).

Shalev, I. (2014). Implicit energy loss: Embodied dryness cues influence vitality and depletion. Journal of Consumer Psychology (in this

issue).

Shen, H., & Sengupta, J. (2012). If you can't grab it, it won't grab you: The effect of restricting the dominant hand on target evaluations. Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 525–529.

Shu, S., & Peck, J. (2011). Psychological ownership and affective reaction: Emotional attachment process variables and the endowment effect. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(4), 439–452.

Simmons, W. K., Martin, A., & Barsalou, L. W. (2005). Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1602–1608.

Smeets, J. B. J., & Brenner, E. (2006). 10 years of illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1501–1504.

Smith, E. R., & Semin, G. R. (2004). Socially situated cognition: Cognition in its social context. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36,

53–117.

Spangenberg, E. R., Crowley, A. E., & Henderson, P. W. (1996). Improving the store environment: Do olfactory cues affect evaluationsand behavior?

Journal of Marketing, 60, 67–80.

Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2007). The role of impulse in social behavior. In A.

Kruglanski,& E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology. Handbook of basic

principles (pp. 408–431) (2nd ed.). NewYork: Guilford.

Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial

feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,

768–777.

Topolinski, S. (2010). Moving the eye of the beholder: Motor components in vision determine aesthetic preference. Psychological Science, 21,

1220–1224.

Topolinski, S. (2012). The sensorimotor contributions to implicit memory, familiarity, and recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(2), 260–281.

Topolinski, S., Lindner, S., & Freudenberg, A. -L. (2013). Popcorn in the cinema: Oral interference sabotages advertising effects. Journal of

Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009). Motormouth: Mere exposure depends on stimulus specific motor simulations. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 423–433.

Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Human Perception and Performance, 24(6), 830–846.

Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2001). The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization. Visual Cognition, 8(6), 769–800.

Wansink, B., & Van Ittersum, K. (2003). Bottoms up! The influence of elongation on pouring and consumption volume. Journal of Consumer

Research, 30(3), 455–463.

Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth influences interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606–607.

Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded

mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of

the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39,

1257–1267.

Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–636.

10 A. Krishna, N. Schwarz / Journal of Consumer Psychology xx, x (2014) xxx–xxx

Wyer, R. S. (1974). Cognitive organization and change: An information processing approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Xu, A. J., & Labroo, A. (2014). Incandescent affect: Turning on the hot emotional system with bright light. Journal of Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

Xu, A. J., Zwick, R., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Washing away your (good or bad) luck: Physical cleansing affects risk-taking behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 26–30.

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph, 9(2), 1–27.Zhong, C. B., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2008). Cold and lonely: Does social exclusion feel literally cold? Psychological Science, 19, 838–842.

Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 1451–1452.

Zillman, D. (1978). Attribution and misattribution of excitatory reactions. In J.

H. Harvey, W. I. Ickes,& R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution

research, Vol. 2. (pp. 335–368). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Zwebner, Y., Lee, L., & Goldenberg, J. (2014). The temperature premium: Warm temperatures increase product valuation. Journal of Consumer Psychology (in this issue).

零售企业营销策略中英文对照外文翻译文献

零售企业营销策略中英文对照外文翻译文献(文档含英文原文和中文翻译)

译文: 零售企业的营销策略 Philip Kotlor 今天的零售商为了招徕和挽留顾客,急欲寻找新的营销策略。过去,他们挽留顾客的方法是销售特别的或独特的花色品种,提供比竞争对手更多更好的服务提供商店信用卡是顾客能赊购商品。可是,现在这一切都已变得面目全非了。现在,诸如卡尔文·克连,依佐和李维等全国性品牌,不仅在大多数百货公司及其专营店可以看到,并且也可以在大型综合商场和折扣商店可以买到。全国性品牌的生产商为全力扩大销售量,它们将贴有品牌的商品到处销售。结果是零售商店的面貌越来越相似。 在服务项目上的分工差异在逐渐缩小。许多百货公司削减了服务项目,而许多折扣商店却增加了服务项目。顾客变成了精明的采购员,对价格更加敏感。他们看不出有什么道理要为相同的品牌付出更多的钱,特别是当服务的差别不大或微不足道时。由于银行信用卡越来越被所有的商家接受,他们觉得不必从每个商店赊购商品。 百货商店面对着日益增加的价格的折扣店和专业商店的竞争,准备东山再起。历史上居于市中心的许多商店在郊区购物中心开设分店,那里有宽敞的停车场,购买者来自人口增长较快并且有较高收入的地区。其他一些则对其商店形式进行改变,有些则试用邮购盒电话订货的方法。超级市场面对的是超级商店的竞争,它们开始扩大店面,经营大量的品种繁多的商品和提高设备等级,超级市场还增加了它们的促销预算,大量转向私人品牌,从而增加盈利。 现在,我们讨论零售商在目标市场、产品品种和采办、服务以及商店气氛、定价、促销和销售地点等方面的营销策略。 一、目标市场 零售商最重要的决策时确定目标市场。当确定目标市场并且勾勒出轮廓时,零售商才能对产品分配、商店装饰、广告词和广告媒体、价格水平等作出一致的决定。如沃尔玛的目标市场相当明确:

英文文献小米手机的发展看市场营销

The marketing of millet phone Chapter I The background of millet Technology Company The millet Company was formally established in April 2010, is an independent research and development to focus on high-end smart phone mobile Internet company. Millet phone, MIUI, m chat millet company's three core businesses. Fever born millet product concept. Millet company pioneered the development of mobile phone operating system with the Internet model, 600,000 enthusiasts to participate in the development and improvement mode. Millet Technology (full name is millet technology limited liability company) set up by the former companies such as Google, Microsoft, Jinshan top players, is a focus on iphone, android, and a new generation of smart phone software development company hot mobile Internet business operations. Officially launched in April 2010, has to obtain the renowned Ambassador investors and venture capital Morningside, the Qiming huge investment. The end of 2010, mobile phone real name Community meters chat launched within six months, registered users exceeded 300 million. In addition, millet company also introduced the CM-based custom mobile operating system MIUI Android dual-core mobile phone millet phone. M chat, miui, millet phone is the the millet technology of three core products. The millet logo is the shape of "MI", is short for Mobile Internet, on behalf of millet is a mobile Internet company. Millet LOGO upside down is a heart word, the means of millet is to let millet user save heart. Millet Pinyin is mi, First Mobile Internet, the millet to do mobile Internet company; Second Mission Impossible, the millet to complete the task can not be completed; course, Lei Jun millet and rifles to conquer the world with his millet team. Chapter II millet phone marketing strategy 2.1 millet mobile phone marketing objectives August 16, 2011, when the millet Technology CEO Lei Jun appeared on the stage of the 798 Art District, Beijing Clubhouse, the presence of rice shouted "Lei Busi" name, this is no doubt that, in the minds of rice flour and look forward to forward Leijun Cheng as the next Steve Jobs, millet hope Apple phone. TechCrunch Disrupt conference held in Beijing on November 1, 2011, the millet Technology CEO Lei Jun said, the the millet technology goal is to become a "world-class" mobile Internet company and successfully broke into the top 500 list of "the rich" Apple iPhone redefined the smart phone and other smart phone products in the past five years, could not keep up with the iPhone for the same reason I launched millet phone. " 2.2 millet mobile product strategy Product strategy companies develop business strategies, we must first clear what products and services to meet the demands of consumers, that is to solve the problem of product strategy. Product strategy is a combination of the core of the marketing 4P, pricing strategy, distribution strategy and promotional strategy based. From the point of view of the socio-economic development, the exchange of products is a necessary prerequisite for the social division of labor, corporate production and social needs of the reunification is achieved through the product, the relationship between the enterprise and the market mainly through the product or service to contact from internal purposes, is the center of the production activities. Therefore, the product strategy is the

毕业论文外文文献翻译Brand-Strategy-Research企业品牌战略研究

毕业设计(论文)外文文献翻译 文献、资料中文题目:企业品牌战略研究 文献、资料英文题目:Brand Strategy Research 文献、资料来源: 文献、资料发表(出版)日期: 院(部): 专业: 班级: 姓名: 学号: 指导教师: 翻译日期: 2017.02.14

Brand Strategy Research Kapferer,J.H Available online 11 June 2010 Economic globalization,how to adapt to international trends,establish,a s trong brand and enhance our competitiveness,have become pressing issues facing enterprises.Based on the analysis of the development of corporate marketing brand strategy in enterprise marketing role.Enterise needs to sue a variety of means of competition to increase brand awarenss, improve brand positioning, an create a good brand image. First, Japanese brands across the board defeat. November 22,2006 morning, NEC announced that it would withdraw from 2G and 2.5mobile phone market ,which means that, following Sharp, Panasonic, Toshiba, Mitsubishi, Sanyo, a Japanese mobile phone manufacturers later withdraw from the Chinese market, Japanese mobile phone has almost all except Kyocera 2G mobile phone market in China out of contention. If we sum up the Chinese household appliance market, today any different from ten years ago,I think the biggest difference is that Japanese c ompanies in China, Japanese home appliance market downturn, the following main reasons: First, rigid enterprise system, decision-making difficult, the reaction was slow, incompatible with the reality of the Chinese market ,it is difficult to adapt to the rapidly changing Chinese market;2 is weak in marketing ,product planning capacity is not strong ,it is difficult to judge according to their marker lacunch to meet consumer demand and forecast products, follow the trend has been in a passive situation, can not satisfy market demand; Third, failure to grasp the industry best time to transition is the Japanese home appliance companies lose an important reason for market dominance. Japanese companies come to the edge in the Chinese market is causing companies tothink deeply about our nation ? To make the internanational route and whether the enterprise of“Japanese Company”to the lessons learned behind?Second, the brand strategy implementation in China the Current Situation Many old famous“flash in the pen” Chinese and foreign enterprises in the Chinese market the brand war;just grow up to be a great impact on national brands. The last century, a little-know 80’s brand ,not being registered by trademark, is to be acquired, squeeze, even if the residue is hard going down really developed very limited.Here atypical case, the last

外文文献(市场营销策略)

Marketing Strategy Market Segmentation and Target Strategy A market consists of people or organizations with wants,money to spend,and the willingness to spend it.However,within most markets the buyer' needs are not identical.Therefore,a single marketing program starts with identifying the differences that exist within a market,a process called market segmentation, and deciding which segments will be pursued ads target markets. Marketing segmentation enables a company to make more efficient use of its marketing resources.Also,it allows a small company to compete effectively by concentrating on one or two segments.The apparent drawback of market segmentation is that it will result in higher production and marketing costs than a one-product,mass-market strategy.However, if the market is correctly segmented,the better fit with customers' needs will actually result in greater efficiency. The three alternative strategies for selecting a target market are market aggregation,single segment,and multiple segment.Market-aggregation strategy involves using one marketing mix to reach a mass,undifferentiated market.With a single-segment strategy, a company still uses only one marketing mix,but it is directed at only one segment of the total market.A multiple-segment strategy entails selecting two or more segments and developing a separate marketing mix to reach segment. Positioning the Product Management's ability to bring attention to a product and to differentiate it in a favorable way from similar products goes a long way toward determining that product's revenues.Thus management needs to engage in positioning,which means developing the image that a product projects in relation to competitive products and to the firm's other products. Marketing executives can choose from a variety of positioning strategies.Sometimes they decide to use more than one for a particular product.Here are several major positioning strategies:

市场营销价格策略外文翻译文献

市场营销价格策略外文翻译文献(文档含英文原文和中文翻译)

DESIGNING PRICING STRATEGIES All for-profit organizations and many nonprofit organizations set prices on their goods or services. Whether the price is called rent (for an apartment), tuition (for education), fare (for travel), or interest (for borrowed money), the concept is the same. Throughout most of history, prices were set by negotiation between buyers and sellers. Setting one price for all buyers arose with the development of large-scale retailing at the en d of the nineteenth century, when Woolworth’s and other stores followed a “strictly one-price policy” because they carried so many items and had so many employees. Now, 100 years later, technology is taking us back to an era of negotiated pricing. The Internet, corporate networks, and wireless setups are linking people, machines, and companies around the globe, connecting sellers and buyers as never before. Web sites like https://www.360docs.net/doc/c110303719.html, and https://www.360docs.net/doc/c110303719.html, allow buyers to compare products and prices quickly and easily. On-line auction sites like https://www.360docs.net/doc/c110303719.html, and https://www.360docs.net/doc/c110303719.html, make it easy for buyers and sellers to negotiate prices on thousands of items. At the same time, new tech- nologies are allowing sellers to collect detailed data about customers’ buying habits, preferences—even spending limits—so they can tailor their products and prices. 1 In the entire marketing mix, price is the one element that produces revenue; the others produce costs. Price is also one of the most ?exible elements: It can be changed quickly, unlike product features and channel commitments. Although price competi- tion is a major problem facing companies, many do not handle pricing well. The most common mistakes are these: Pricing is too cost-oriented; price is not revised often

市场营销_外文翻译_外文文献_英文文献_顾客满意策略与顾客满意

顾客满意策略与顾客满意营销 原文来源:《Marketing Customer Satisfaction 》自20世纪八十年代末以来, 顾客满意战略已日益成为各国企业占有更多的顾客份额, 获得竞争优势的整体经营手段。 一、顾客满意策略是现代企业获得顾客“货币选票”的法宝随着时代的变迁, 社会物质财富的极大充裕, 顾客中的主体———消费者的需求也先后跨越了物质缺乏的时代、追求数量的时代、追求品质的时代, 到了20世纪八十年代末进入了情感消费时代。在我国, 随着经济的高速发展,我们也已迅速跨越了物质缺乏时代、追求数量的时代乃至追求品质的时代, 到今天也逐步迈进情感消费时代。在情感消费时代, 各企业的同类产品早已达到同时、同质、同能、同价, 消费者追求的已不再是质量、功能和价格, 而是舒适、便利、安全、安心、速度、跃动、环保、清洁、愉快、有趣等,消费者日益关注的是产品能否为自己的生活带来活力、充实、舒适、美感和精神文化品位, 以及超越消费者期望值的售前、售中、售后服务和咨询。也就是说, 今天人们所追求的是具有“心的满足感和充实感”的商品, 是高附加值的商品和服务,追求价值观和意识多元化、个性化和无形的满足感的时代已经来临。 与消费者价值追求变化相适应的企业间的竞争, 也由产品竞争、价格竞争、技术竞争、广告竞争、品牌竞争发展到现今的形象竞争、信誉竞争、文化竞争和服务竞争, 即顾客满意竞争。这种竞争是企业在广角度、宽领域的时空范围内展开的高层次、体现综合实力的竞争。它包括组织创新力、技术创新力、管理创新力、产业预见力、产品研发力、员工向心力、服务顾客力、顾客亲和力、同行认同力、社会贡献力、公关传播沟通力、企业文化推动力、环境适应力等等。这些综合形象力和如何合成综合持久的竞争力, 这就是CSft略所要解决的问题。CS寸代,企业不再以“自己为中心”,而是以“顾客为中心”;“顾客为尊”、“顾客满意”不再是流于形式的口号, 而是以实实在在的行动为基础的企业经营的一门新哲学。企业不再以质量达标, 自己满意为经营理念, 而是以顾客满意, 赢得顾客高忠诚度为经营理念。企业经营策略的焦点不再以争取或保持市场占有率为主, 而是以争取顾客满意为经营理念。因此, 营销策略的重心不再放在竞争对手身上而是放在顾客身上, 放在顾客现实的、潜在的需求上。当企业提供的产品和服务达到了顾客事先的期望值, 顾客就基本满意;如果远远超越顾客的期望值, 且远远高于其他同行, 顾客才真正满意;如果企业能不断地或长久地令顾客满意, 顾客就会忠诚。忠诚的顾客不仅会经常性地重复购买, 还会购买企业其它相关的产品或服务;忠诚的顾客不仅会积极向别人推荐他所买的产品, 而且对企业竞争者的促销活动具有免疫能力一个不满意的顾客会将不满意告诉16-20个人, 而每一个被告知者会再传播给12-15个人。这样, 一个不满意者会影响到二、三百人。在互联网普及的今天, 其影响则更大。据美国汽车业的调查, 一个满意者会引发8笔潜在的生意, 其中至少有一笔会成交。而另一项调查表明, 企业每增加5%的忠诚顾客, 利润就会增长25%-95%。一个企业的80%的利润来自20%的忠诚顾客;而获取一个新顾客的成本是维持一个老顾客成本的6倍。所以,美国著名学者唐?佩 珀斯指出: 决定一个企业成功与否的关键不是市场份额, 而是在于顾客份额。 于是, 企业纷纷通过广泛细致的市场调研、与消费者直接接触、顾客信息反馈等方式来了解顾客在各方面的现实需求和潜在需求。依靠对企业满意忠诚的销售、服务人员, 定期、定量地对顾客满意度进行综合测定, 以便准确地把握企业经营中与“顾客满意” 目标的差距及其重点领域, 从而进一步改善企业的经营活动。依靠高亲和力的企业文化、高效率的人文管理和全员共同努力, 不断地向顾客提供高附加值的产品, 高水准的亲情般的服

品牌营销战略参考文献和英文文献翻译

品牌营销战略参考文献和英文文献翻译 目录外文文献翻译..............................................................................................1 摘要..........................................................................................................1 1. 品牌战略内涵与其功能意义.......................................................................2 2. 我国企业品牌发展概况..............................................................................3 2.1 国内品牌与国外品牌相比存在着很大的差距............................................3 2.2 品牌发展缺乏整体规划. (4) 2.3 产品质量低下品牌个性不足缺乏创新和发展能力.....................................4 2.4 品牌发展策略存在误区. (4) 3. 企业品牌策略选择..................................................................................6 3.1 树立正确的品牌竞争意识着力提高品牌竞争能力......................................6 3.2 搞好品牌定位培养消费者品牌偏好与品牌忠诚.. (6) 3.3 遵循品牌设计规律注重品牌形

市场营销策略外文文献及翻译

市场营销策略外文文献及翻译 Marketing Strategy Market Segmentation and Target Strategy A market consists of people or organizations with wants,money to spend,and the willingness to spend it.However,within most markets the buyer' needs are not identical.Therefore,a single marketing program starts with identifying the differences that exist within a market,a process called market segmentation, and deciding which segments will be pursued ads target markets. Marketing segmentation enables a company to make more efficient use of its marketing resources.Also,it allows a small company to compete effectively by concentrating on one or two segments.The apparent drawback of market segmentation is that it will result in higher production and marketing costs than a one-product,mass-market strategy.However, if the market is correctly segmented,the better fit with customers' needs will actually result in greater efficiency. The three alternative strategies for selecting a target market are market aggregation,single segment,and multiple segment.Market-aggregation strategy involves using one marketing mix to reach a mass,undifferentiated market.With a single-segment strategy, a company still uses only one marketing mix,but it is directed at only one segment of the total market.A multiple-segment strategy entails

差异化营销策略外文文献翻译

文献出处: Dalman, M. Deniz, and Junhong Min. "Marketing Strategy for Unusual Brand Differentiation: Trivial Attribute Effect." International Journal of Marketing Studies 6.5 (2014): 63-72. 原文 Marketing Strategy for Unusual Brand Differentiation: Trivial Attribute Effect Dalman, M. Deniz & Junhong Min Abstract This research investigates that brand differentiation creating superior values can be achieved not only by adding meaningful attributes but also meaningless attributes, which is called "trivial attribute effect." Two studies provided empirical evidences as following; first, trivial attribute effect creates a strong brand differentiation even after subjects realize that trivial attribute has no value. Second, trivial attribute effect is more pronounced in hedonic service category compared to the utilitarian category. Last, the amount of willingness to pay is higher when trivial attribute is presented and evaluated in joint evaluation mode than separate evaluation mode. Finally, we conclude with discussion and provide suggestions for further research. Keywords: brand differentiation, evaluation mode, service industry, trivial attribute Introduction Problem Definition Perhaps the most important factor for new product success is to create the meaningful brand differentiation that provides customers with superior values beyond what the competitors can offer in the same industry (Porter, 1985). Not surprisingly, more than 50 percent of annual sales in consumer product industries including automobiles, biotechnology, computer software, and pharmaceuticals are attributed to such meaningful brand differentiation by including new or noble attributes (Schilling &Hill, 1998). However, the brand differentiation that increases consumer preference is not only by introducing meaningful attributes but also meaningless attributes. For

品牌国际化外文翻译文献

品牌国际化外文翻译文献(文档含英文原文和中文翻译)

企业品牌战略研究 在经济全球化的今天,如何适应国际化潮流,建立强势品牌,提高竞争能力,已经成为国内企业面临的迫切问题。本文在分析我国企业营销品牌战略发展状况的基础上,从品牌战略的内涵与其功能意义入手,探讨了品牌战略在企业营销中的作用。企业需要综合运用多种竞争手段提高品意,搞好品牌定位,塑造良好品牌形象。 一、日系品牌全线崩溃 2006年11月22日上午,NEC宣布将推出2G及2.5G手机市场,这意味着继夏普、松下、东芝、三菱、三洋之后又一家日本手机厂商退出中国市场,日系手机除京瓷外几乎全部退出中国2G手机市场的争夺。 如果我们总结今天的中国家电市场与十年前有什么不同的话,我想,最大的不同就是,日系企业在中国的繁荣已经渐行渐远。 对于日系手机败退,乃至日系家电走到中国市场的低谷,主要原因有以下几点:一是企业制度呆板,决策困难,反应速度慢,与另市场现实格格不入,难以适应快速变化的中国市场;二是市场营销能力弱,产品规划能力不强,很难根椐自己对市场的判断与预测推出迎合消费需求的产品,一直处于跟风的被动局面,无法满足中国市场的需要;三是未能把握住产业转型最佳时机,是日系家电企业失去市场主导地位的重要原因。 日系企业在中国市场上走到边缘是否引起我们民族企业的深思?欲走国际化路线的企业又是否从“日系企业”的背后吸取教训? 二、我国企业实施品牌战略的现状分析处 第一,众多昔日名牌“昙花一现”,中外企业在市场上的品牌大战,使刚刚成长起来的民族品牌受到极大的冲击。上世纪80年代稍有知名度的品牌,不是被抢注商标,就是被收购、挤垮,即使残留下来的也是惨淡经营,真正发展起来的极为有限。这里典型的案例,上世纪80年代至90年代初期,曾在空调界创下奇迹的华宝空调,在1998年被科龙收购,其后的品牌形象就一再下滑。 第二,品牌战略已日渐引起国内企业重视,获得政府的扶持。自上世纪80年代改革以来,我国社会主义经济建设取得了令人瞩目的成就,从计划经济时代走向市场经济时代的中国企业,品牌经营无从无到有。

市场营销战略论文中英文外文翻译文献

中英文外文翻译文献 消费者行为与市场营销战略 消费者行为学是研究个体,群体和组织为满足其需要而如何选择,获取,使用,处置产品,服务,体验和想法以及由此对消费者和社会产生的影响.传统上,消费者行为研究侧重于购买前和购买后的有关活动.关于消费者行为学的界定较之传统观点更广泛,它将有助于引导我们从更宽广的视角审视消费者决策的间接影响以及对买卖双方的各种后果.为了在竞争激烈的环境中求得生存,企业必须比竞争者更多地为目标客户提供价值.顾客价值是顾客从整体产品中获得的各项利益扣除各种获取费用后的余额. 1.市场营销战略 对每一选取的目标市场,都应分别制定营销战略.选择目标市场的关键性标准或依据是企业是否有能力提供较竞争品高的消费者价值.消费者价值很大程度上是由营销战略决定,所以公司在评估潜在目标市场时,应当发展一般的营销战略.

1.1 产品 产品是消费者获得和用于满足其需要的任何东西.消费者所购买的或追求的是需要的满足,而不是具体形态的物质特性. 1.2 传播 营销传播包括广告,人员分销,公共关系,包装以及企业提供的关于它自身及其产品的其他信号. 1.3 定价 价格是消费者为获得拥有,使用产品的权利而必须支付的金钱数量.消 费者可以拥有一件产品,也可以仅仅拥有产品的使用权. 1.4 分销 分销实际上是让顾客在需要的时候能买到产品,它对企业的经营成败至关重要.绝大多数情况下,消费者不愿为获得某一特定品牌而伤身费力.很明显,有效地渠道决策应即建立在掌握消费者在何处购买的知识的基础上. 2.市场分析 市场分析要求全面深入地了解企业自身能力,现在和潜在竞争者的实力,潜在消费者的消费过程以及经济的,物质的和技术的环境. 2.1 消费者 不了解消费者,就无法预测其需要与欲望,也无法对其需要做出恰当的 反映.发现消费者现在需要什么是一个复杂的过程,但一般来说,可以通过直接的营销调研予以实现 2.2 公司 每一个公司都必须透彻了解其满足消费者需要的能力.为此,需要评价 公司的各个方面,如财务状况,一般管理技巧,研究和开发能力,技术装

网络营销策略外文文献翻译

文献出处:Guzzo T, D’Andrea A, Ferri F, et al. Evolution of Marketing Strategies: From Internet Marketing to M-Marketing[C]//On the Move to Meaningful Internet Systems: OTM 2012 Workshops. Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2012: 627-636. 原文 Evolution of Marketing Strategies:From Internet Marketing to M-Marketing Tiziana Guzzo, Alessia D’Andrea, Fernando Ferri, and Patrizia Grifoni 1 Introduction Marketing is “the process which creates, communicates, delivers the value to the consumers, and maintains the relationship with consumers. It generates the strategy that underlies sales techniques, business communication, and business developments. It is “an integrated process through which companies build strong consumers relationships and create value for their consumers and for themselves” Marketing strategies have a long history and mainly interested economists and sociologists. The wide use of Internet, pervasiveness of social networks and the evolution of mobile devise are implying a wider involvement of interdisciplinary competences enlarging the interest toward ICT competences. This paper is to describe the evolution of marketing strategies from the advent of the Web (Internet Marketing) - through the advent of Social Networks (Marketing 2.0) - to the evolution of Mobile Social Networks (M-marketing). In particular, the paper analyses the use that Italian people make of mobile devices and the user perception and acceptance of M-marketing.

相关文档
最新文档