What About Negation in Spatial Reasoning
为什么人们有容貌焦虑英语作文

The Roots of Appearance Anxiety: AnExplorationIn today's society, the pressure to conform to aesthetic standards is immense, leading many individuals to experience anxiety about their appearance. This phenomenon, known as appearance anxiety, is a complex issue that stems from various sources. This essay delves into the reasons why people develop appearance anxiety, examining societal pressures, social media influence, personal insecurities, and psychological factors.Firstly, societal pressures play a significant role in shaping our perceptions of beauty. From childhood, we are constantly bombarded with messages about what is considered attractive and what is not. These messages are often transmitted through media, advertising, and even by our peers and family members. As a result, many individuals internalize these standards, believing that they must meet them to be accepted and loved.Secondly, the rise of social media has further exacerbated this issue. With platforms like Instagram and TikTok becoming increasingly popular, people are constantlybombarded with images of "perfect" bodies, faces, and lifestyles. This constant exposure to unrealistic and often edited representations of beauty can make individuals feel inadequate and inferior.Moreover, personal insecurities can also contribute to appearance anxiety. Whether it's a childhood trauma, a critical comment from a loved one, or a constant comparison to others, these experiences can leave lasting impacts on our self-esteem. When individuals lack confidence in their appearance, they may develop anxiety about being judged or rejected based on their looks.Lastly, psychological factors such as body image distortion and low self-esteem can also lead to appearance anxiety. Body image distortion is a condition where individuals have a negative or distorted perception oftheir body size, shape, or appearance. This can lead to feelings of shame, guilt, and anxiety about one's appearance. Low self-esteem, on the other hand, can make individuals doubt their worth and value, leading them to believe that their appearance is their only redeeming quality.In conclusion, appearance anxiety is a complex issue that arises from multiple factors. From societal pressures to personal insecurities and psychological factors, the reasons why people develop this anxiety are diverse and interconnected. It is important to remember that beauty is subjective and that everyone deserves to feel confident and comfortable in their own skin. By challenging societal standards, promoting body positivity, and fostering self-love, we can create a more inclusive and accepting society where appearance anxiety is less prevalent.**容貌焦虑的根源:一次探索**在当今社会,符合审美标准的压力巨大,导致许多人对自己的容貌感到焦虑。
优秀英语美文摘抄并赏析

优秀英语美文摘抄并赏析优秀英语美文摘抄并赏析英语美文题材丰富,涉及面广,大多蕴涵人生哲理。
引导学生欣赏美文,不仅能提高他们的阅读理解能力,而且能使他们得到美的熏陶,从而提高学生对周围事物的认识。
本文是优秀英语美文,希望对大家有帮助! 优秀英语美文:The Love of BeautyThe love of beauty is an essential part of all healthy human nature. It is a moral quality. The absence of it is not an assured ground of condemnation, but the presence of it is an invariable sign of goodness of heart. In proportion to the degree in which it is felt will probably be the degree in which nobleness and beauty of character will be attained.Natural beauty is an all-pervading presence. The universe is its temple. It unfolds into the numberless flowers of spring. It waves in the branches of trees and the green blades of grass. It haunts the depths of the earth and the sea. It gleams from the hues of the shell and the precious stone. And not only these minute objects but the oceans, the mountains, the clouds, the stars, the rising and the setting sun---all overflow with beauty. This beauty is so precious, and so congenial to our tenderest and noblest feelings, that it is painful to think of the multitude of people living in the midst of it and yet remaining almost blind to it.All persons should seek to become acquainted with the beauty in nature. There is not a worm we tread upon, nor a leaf that dances merrily as it falls before the autumn winds, but calls for our study and admiration. The power to appreciated beauty not merely increases our sources of happiness---it enlarges our moral nature, too. Beauty calms our restlessness and dispels ourcares. Go into the fields or the woods, spend a summer day by the sea or the mountains, and all your little perplexities and anxieties will vanish. Listen to sweet music, and your foolish fears and petty jealousies will pass away. The beauty of the world helps us to seek and find the beauty of goodness.爱美爱美及是整个健全人性不可或缺之一部分。
不自信的原因作文英语作文

不自信的原因作文英语作文In our daily lives, we often encounter individuals who seemto lack confidence. This lack of self-assurance can manifestin various ways, such as hesitancy in speaking up, reluctance to take risks, or a general sense of inadequacy. There are several reasons why people might feel unconfident, and understanding these can be the first step towards building a stronger sense of self.Low Self-Esteem:A common cause of lack of confidence is low self-esteem. This can stem from negative self-talk or from the constant comparison with others. When individuals believe they are not good enough, they may doubt their abilities and hesitate to take on new challenges.Past Failures:Previous failures can also contribute to a lack of confidence. When people experience setbacks or failures, they may internalize these as a reflection of their abilities, leading to a fear of trying again and a diminished belief in theirown capabilities.Fear of Judgment:The fear of being judged by others is another significant factor. Many people are hesitant to express their thoughts or take actions because they worry about criticism or rejection from their peers.Lack of Experience:Inexperience can lead to uncertainty and doubt. When individuals have not had the opportunity to develop skills or gain knowledge in a particular area, they may feel unprepared and, consequently, lack confidence.Overprotective Environment:Growing up in an overprotective environment can hinder the development of self-confidence. If individuals are not given the chance to face challenges and learn from their mistakes, they may grow up feeling incapable of handling difficulties on their own.Social Anxiety:Social anxiety is another reason for low confidence. People who suffer from social anxiety often feel extremely self-conscious and anxious in social situations, which can lead to avoidance of these situations and a lack of confidence in social interactions.Physical Appearance:Concerns about physical appearance can also impact one's confidence. In a society that often places a high value on looks, those who feel they do not meet certain beauty standards may struggle with self-esteem and confidence.Lack of Support:Finally, a lack of support from family, friends, or mentors can contribute to a lack of confidence. Encouragement and positive reinforcement play a crucial role in building self-assurance.To overcome these issues and build confidence, individuals can take steps such as setting achievable goals, seeking support from positive influences, and practicing self-compassion. It is also important to challenge negative thoughts and to celebrate successes, no matter how small they may seem. With time and effort, confidence can be nurtured and developed, leading to a more fulfilling and successful life.。
大学生缺乏批判精神的原因英语作文

大学生缺乏批判精神的原因英语作文全文共3篇示例,供读者参考篇1Title: The Lack of Critical Thinking in College StudentsIn recent years, there has been a growing concern about the lack of critical thinking skills among college students. Critical thinking is the ability to analyze and evaluate information objectively, as well as to think independently and solve problems creatively. However, many students seem to be lacking this important skill. In this essay, we will explore the reasons behind the lack of critical thinking in college students.One of the main reasons for the lack of critical thinking skills in college students is the emphasis on rote memorization in the education system. Students are often taught to memorize facts and information without questioning or analyzing them critically. This approach discourages students from thinking independently and critically about the material they are learning. Consequently, students may struggle to apply their knowledge to real-world situations and to think creatively and critically about complex issues.Another factor contributing to the lack of critical thinking in college students is the prevalence of social media and technology. With the rise of social media platforms and smartphones, students are constantly bombarded with information and opinions, often without the opportunity to critically evaluate them. This constant stream of information can make it difficult for students to develop their own opinions and think critically about complex issues.Moreover, the pressure to succeed academically and the focus on grades and test scores can also hinder the development of critical thinking skills. Students may be more focused on getting the right answer rather than on understanding the underlying concepts and thinking critically about them. This can lead to a surface-level understanding of the material and an inability to think critically about complex issues.Additionally, the lack of exposure to diverse perspectives and viewpoints can also contribute to the lack of critical thinking in college students. Many students may only be exposed to ideas and opinions that align with their own beliefs, which can limit their ability to think critically about differing viewpoints. Without exposure to diverse perspectives, students may struggle to thinkcritically about complex issues and to consider alternative solutions.In conclusion, there are several reasons behind the lack of critical thinking skills in college students, including the emphasis on rote memorization, the influence of social media and technology, the pressure to succeed academically, and the lack of exposure to diverse perspectives. To address this issue, educators and policymakers need to prioritize the development of critical thinking skills in the education system and provide students with the tools and opportunities to think independently and critically about the world around them. By fostering a culture of critical thinking, we can empower college students to analyze information objectively, think creatively, and solve problems effectively.篇2Title: Lack of Critical Thinking among College StudentsIn today's society, critical thinking is an essential skill that allows individuals to analyze information, evaluate arguments, and make rational decisions. However, many college students are found to lack this crucial skill, leading to difficulties in problem-solving, decision-making, and overall intellectualgrowth. In this essay, we will explore the reasons behind the lack of critical thinking among college students and suggest ways to address this issue.One of the main reasons for the lack of critical thinking among college students is the education system itself. Many traditional educational methods focus on rote memorization and regurgitation of facts, rather than encouraging students to question, analyze, and critique information. This leads to a passive learning environment where students are unable to develop their critical thinking skills.Another major factor is the reliance on technology and social media. With the abundance of information available at their fingertips, students often accept information at face value without questioning its validity or reliability. This leads to a lack of skepticism and critical evaluation of sources, which are essential components of critical thinking.Furthermore, the pressure to perform well academically and secure good grades can hinder the development of critical thinking skills. Many students prioritize memorization over understanding, as they are more concerned with achieving high marks than engaging in meaningful and thoughtful analysis.Moreover, the lack of diversity in perspectives and experiences within educational institutions can inhibit critical thinking. When students are not exposed to a variety of viewpoints and ideas, they may struggle to think critically about complex issues and develop their own informed opinions.To address the issue of the lack of critical thinking among college students, educational institutions need to reform their teaching methods to emphasize critical thinking skills. This can be achieved through interactive and collaborative learning activities, such as debates, case studies, and problem-solving exercises, that encourage students to think critically and engage actively with the material.In addition, educators should incorporate media literacy skills into the curriculum to help students navigate the vast amount of information available online and develop the ability to critically evaluate sources. This can help students become more discerning consumers of information and avoid falling prey to misinformation and fake news.Furthermore, fostering a culture of intellectual curiosity and open-mindedness within educational institutions can help stimulate critical thinking among students. Encouraging students to explore different perspectives, challenge their assumptions,and engage in respectful debates can help them develop their critical thinking skills and become more well-rounded individuals.In conclusion, the lack of critical thinking among college students is a concerning issue that requires immediate attention. By addressing the root causes of this problem and implementing strategies to promote critical thinking skills, we can equip students with the tools they need to become active and engaged learners, critical thinkers, and informed citizens.篇3Title: The Lack of Critical Thinking Among College StudentsIntroductionIn today's society, critical thinking is an essential skill that every individual should possess. However, it is often observed that many college students lack this critical thinking ability. This issue is concerning as critical thinking is necessary for problem-solving, decision-making, and overall intellectual growth. In this essay, we will explore the reasons behind the lack of critical thinking among college students.1. Overemphasis on rote learningOne of the main reasons for the lack of critical thinking among college students is the overemphasis on rote learning in the education system. Students are often forced to memorize facts and information without truly understanding the underlying concepts. This leads to a passive approach to learning, where students simply regurgitate information without questioning or analyzing it critically.2. Influences of social media and technologyAnother factor contributing to the lack of critical thinking among college students is the influence of social media and technology. With the widespread use of smartphones and social media platforms, students are constantly bombarded with information and opinions. This constant flow of information can lead to a superficial understanding of complex issues and hinder the development of critical thinking skills.3. Pressure to succeed academicallyCollege students are often under immense pressure to succeed academically. This pressure can deter them from engaging in critical thinking, as they are more focused on achieving high grades and meeting performance standards. As a result, many students prioritize memorization and rote learningover critical thinking, as it is seen as a more efficient way to succeed in academics.4. Lack of exposure to diverse perspectivesCritical thinking involves considering multiple perspectives and viewpoints before forming an opinion or making a decision. However, many college students are limited in their exposure to diverse perspectives due to social and cultural factors. Without exposure to different ideas and beliefs, students may struggle to develop their critical thinking skills and become moreclosed-minded in their approach to problem-solving.5. Lack of encouragement from educatorsFinally, the lack of encouragement from educators can also contribute to the lack of critical thinking among college students. Some educators may prioritize rote learning and memorization over critical thinking skills, leading students to believe that these skills are not important. Additionally, some educators may not provide the necessary guidance and support for students to develop their critical thinking abilities.ConclusionIn conclusion, the lack of critical thinking among college students is a concerning issue that needs to be addressed. Byunderstanding the reasons behind this lack of critical thinking, educators and policymakers can take steps to promote critical thinking skills among college students. Encouraging active learning, providing exposure to diverse perspectives, and fostering a culture of questioning and analyzing information are some ways to cultivate critical thinking among college students. Ultimately, developing critical thinking skills is essential for the intellectual growth and success of college students in today's complex and dynamic world.。
2023年山东省高考英语真题及答案解析

2023年山东省高考英语真题及答案解析本试卷共12页。
考试结束后, 将本试卷和答题卡一并交回。
注意事项: 1. 答题前, 考生先将自己的姓名、准考证号码填写清楚, 将条形码准确粘贴在考生信息条形码粘贴区。
2. 选择题必须使用2B铅笔填涂; 非选择题必须使用0.5毫米黑色字迹的签字笔书写, 字体工整、笔迹清楚。
3. 请按照题号顺序在答题卡各题目的答题区域内作答, 超出答题区域书写的答案无效; 在草稿纸、试卷上答题无效。
4. 作图可先使用铅笔画出, 确定后必须用黑色字迹的签字笔描黑。
5. 保持卡面清洁, 不要折叠, 不要弄破、弄皱, 不准使用涂改液、修正带、刮纸刀。
第一部分听力(1-20小题)在笔试结束后进行。
第二部分阅读(共两节,满分50分)第一节(共15小题;每小题2.5分,满分37.5分)阅读下列短文,从每题所给的A、B、C、D四个选项中选出最佳选项。
ABike Rental & Guided ToursWelcome to Amsterdam, welcome to MacBike. You see much more from the seat of a bike! Cycling is the most economical, sustainable and fun way to explore the city, with its beautiful canals, parks, squares and countless lights. You can also bike along lovely landscapes outside of Amsterdam.Why MacBikeMacBike has been around for almost 30 years and is the biggest bicycle rental company in Amsterdam. With over 2,500 bikes stored in our five rental shops at strategic locations, we make sure there is always a bike available for you. We offer the newest bicycles in a wide variety, including basic bikes with foot brake (刹车), bikes with hand brake and gears (排挡), bikes with child seats, and children’s bikes.PricesGuided City ToursThe 2.5-hour tour covers the Gooyer Windmill, the Skinny Bridge, the Rijksmuseum, Heineken Brewery and much more. The tour departs from Dam Square every hour on the hour, starting at 1:00 pm every day. You can buy your ticket in a MacBike shop or book online.1. What is an advantage of MacBike?A. It gives children a discount.B. It of offers many types of bikes.C. It organizes free cycle tours.D. It has over 2,500 rental shops.2. How much do you pay for renting a bike with hand brake and three gears for two days?A. €15.75.B. €19.50.C. €22.75.D. €29.50.3. Where does the guided city tour start?A. The Gooyer, Windmill.B. The Skinny Bridge.C. Heineken Brewery.D. Dam Square.BWhen John Todd was a child, he loved to explore the woods around his house, observing how nature solved problems. A dirty stream, for example, often became clear after flowing through plants and along rocks where tiny creatures lived. When he got older, John started to wonder if this process could be used to clean up the messes people were making.After studying agriculture, medicine, and fisheries in college, John went back to observing nature and asking questions. Why can certain plants trap harmful bacteria (细菌)? Which kinds of fish can eat cancer-causing chemicals? With the right combination of animals and plants, he figured, maybe he could clean up waste the way nature did. He decided to build what he would later call an eco-machine.The task John set for himself was to remove harmful substances from some sludge (污泥). First, he constructed a series of clear fiberglass tanks connected to each other. Then he went around to local ponds and streams and brought back some plants and animals. He placed them in the tanks and waited. Little by little, these different kinds of life got used to one another and formed their own ecosystem. After a few weeks, John added the sludge.He was amazed at the results. The plants and animals in the eco-machine took the sludge as food and began to eat it! Within weeks it had all been digested, and all that was left was pure water.Over the years, John has taken on many big jobs. He developed a greenhouse — like facility that treated sewage (污水) from 1,600 homes in South Burlington. He also designed an eco-machine to clean canal water in Fuzhou, a city in southeast China.“Ecological design” is the name John gives to what he does. “Life on Earth is kind of a box of spare parts for the inventor,” he says. “You put organisms in new relationships and observe what’s happening. Then you let these new systems develop their own ways to self-repair.”4. What can we learn about John from the first two paragraphs?A. He was fond of traveling.B. He enjoyed being alone.C. He had an inquiring mind.D. He longed to be a doctor.5. Why did John put the sludge into the tanks?A. To feed the animals.B. To build an ecosystem.C. To protect the plants.D. To test the eco-machine.6. What is the author’s purpose in mentioning Fuzhou?A. To review John’s research plans.B. T o show an application of John’s idea.C. To compare John’s different jobs.D. To erase doubts about John’s invention.7. What is the basis for John’s work?A. Nature can repair itself.B. Organisms need water to survive.C. Life on Earth is diverse.D. Most tiny creatures live in groups.CThe goal of this book is to make the case for digital minimalism, including a detailed exploration of what it asks and why it works, and then to teach you how to adopt this philosophy if you decide it’s right for you.To do so, I divided the book into two parts. In part one, I describe the philosophical foundations of digital minimalism, starting with an examination of the forces that are making so many people’s digital lives increasingly intolerable, before moving on to a detailed discussion of the digital minimalism philosophy.Part one concludes by introducing my suggested method for adopting this philosophy: the digital declutter. This process requires you to step away from optional online activities for thirty days. At the end of the thirty days, you will then add back a small number of carefully chosen online activities that you believe will provide massive benefits to the things you value.In the final chapter of part one, I’ll guide you through carrying out your own digital declutter. In doing so, I’ll draw on an experiment I ran in 2018 in which over 1,600 people agreed to perform a digital declutter. You’ll hear these participants’ stories and learn what strategies worked well for them, and what traps they encountered that you should avoid.The second part of this book takes a closer look at some ideas that will help you cultivate (培养) a sustainable digital minimalism lifestyle. In these chapters, I examine issues such as the importance of solitude (独处) and the necessity of cultivating high-quality leisure to replace the time most now spend on mindless device use. Each chapter concludes with a collection of practices, which are designed to help you act on the big ideas of the chapter. You can view these practices as a toolbox meant to aid your efforts to build a minimalist lifestyle that words for your particular circumstances.8. What is the book aimed at?A. Teaching critical thinking skills.B. Advocating a simple digital lifestyle.C. Solving philosophical problems.D. Promoting the use of a digital device.9. What does the underlined word “declutter” in paragraph 3 mean?A. Clear-up.B. Add-on.C. Check-in.D.Take-over.10. What is presented in the final chapter of part one?A. Theoretical models.B. Statistical methods.C. Practical examples.D. Historical analyses.11. What does the author suggest readers do with the practices offered in part two?A. Use them as needed.B. Recommend them to friends.C. Evaluate their effects.D. Identify the ideas behind them.DOn March 7, 1907, the English statistician Francis Galton published a paper which illustrated what has come to be known as the “wisdom of crowds” effect. The experiment of estimation he conducted showed that in some cases, the average of a large number of independent estimates could be quite accurate.This effect capitalizes on the fact that when people make errors, those errors aren’t always the same. Some people will tend to overestimate, and some to underestimate. When enough of these errors are averaged together, they cancel each other out, resulting in a more accurate estimate. If people are similar and tend to make the same errors, then their errors won’t cancel each other out. In more technical terms, the wisdom of crowds requires that people’s estima tes be independent. If for whatever reasons, people’s errors become correlated or dependent, the accuracy of the estimate will go down.But a new study led by Joaquin Navajas offered an interesting twist (转折) on this classic phenomenon. The key finding of the study was that when crowds were further divided into smaller groups that were allowed to have a discussion, the averages from these groups were more accurate than those from an equal number of independent individuals. For instance, the average obtained from the estimates of four discussion groups of five was significantly more accurate than the average obtained from 20 independent individuals.In a follow-up study with 100 university students, the researchers tried to get a better sense of what the group members actually did in their discussion. Did they tend to go with those most confident about their estimates? Did they follow those least willing to change their minds? This happened some of the time, but it wasn’t the dominant response. Most frequently, the groups reported that they “shared arguments and reasoned together.” Somehow, these arguments and reasoning resulted in a global reduction in error. Although the studies led by Navajas have limitations and many questions remain the potential implications for group discussion and decision-making are enormous.12. What is paragraph 2 of the text mainly about?A. The methods of estimation.B. The underlying logic of the effect.C. The causes of people’s errors.D. The design of Galton’s experiment.13. Na vajas’ study found that the average accuracy could increase even if ________.A. the crowds were relatively smallB. there were occasional underestimatesC. individuals did not communicateD. estimates were not fully independent14. What did the follow-up study focus on?A. The size of the groups.B. The dominant members.C. The discussion process.D. The individual estimates.15. What is the author’s attitude toward Navajas’ studies?A. Unclear.B. Dismissive.C. Doubtful.D. Approving.第二节(共5小题;每小题2.5分,满分12.5分)阅读下面短文,从短文后的选项中选出可以填入空白处的最佳选项。
生而为赢美文30篇英文文本及中文翻译

生而为赢美文30篇英文文本及中文翻译引言生而为赢——新东方英语背诵美文30篇目录:•第一篇:Youth 青春•第二篇:Three Days to See(Excerpts)假如给我三天光明(节选)•第三篇:Companionship of Books 以书为伴(节选)•第四篇:If I Rest, I Rust 如果我休息,我就会生锈•第五篇:Ambition 抱负•第六篇:What I have Lived for 我为何而生•第七篇:When Love Beckons You 爱的召唤•第八篇:The Road to Success 成功之道•第九篇:On Meeting the Celebrated 论见名人•第十篇:The 50-Percent Theory of Life 生活理论半对半•第十一篇:What is Your Recovery Rate? 你的恢复速率是多少?•第十二篇:Clear Your Mental Space 清理心灵的空间•第十三篇:Be Happy 快乐•第十四篇:The Goodness of life 生命的美好•第十五篇:Facing the Enemies Within 直面内在的敌人•第十六篇:Abundance is a Life Style 富足的生活方式•第十七篇:Human Life a Poem 人生如诗•第十八篇:Solitude 独处•第十九篇:Giving Life Meaning 给生命以意义•第二十篇:Relish the Moment 品位现在•第二十一篇:The Love of Beauty 爱美•第二十二篇:The Happy Door 快乐之门•第二十三篇:Born to Win 生而为赢•第二十四篇:Work and Pleasure 工作和娱乐•第二十五篇:Mirror, Mirror--What do I see镜子,镜子,告诉我•第二十六篇:On Motes and Beams 微尘与栋梁•第二十七篇:An October Sunrise 十月的日出•第二十八篇:To Be or Not to Be 生存还是毁灭•第二十九篇:Gettysburg Address 葛底斯堡演说•第三十篇:First Inaugural Address(Excerpts) 就职演讲(节选)•第一篇:Youth 青春YouthYouth is not a time of life; it is a state of mind; it is not a matter of rosy cheeks, red lips and supple knees; it is a matter of the will, a quality of the imagination, a vigor of the emotions; it is the freshness of the deep springs of life.Youth means a temperamental predominance of courage over timidity, of the appetite for adventure over the love of ease. This often exists in a man of 60 more than a boy of 20. Nobody grows old merely by a number of years. We grow old by deserting our ideals.Years may wrinkle the skin, but to give up enthusiasm wrinkles the soul. Worry, fear, self-distrust bows the heart and turns the spirit back to dust.Whether 60 or 16, there is in every human being’s he art the lure of wonders, the unfailing appetite for what’s next and the joy of the game of living. In the center of your heart and my heart, there is a wireless station; so long as it receives messages of beauty, hope, courage and power from man and from the infinite, so long as you are young.When your aerials are down, and your spirit is covered with snows of cynicism and the ice of pessimism, then you’ve grown old, even at 20; but as long as your aerials are up, to catch waves of optimism, there’s hope you may die young at 80.译文:青春青春不是年华,而是心境;青春不是桃面、丹唇、柔膝,而是深沉的意志,恢宏的想象,炙热的恋情;青春是生命的深泉在涌流。
英语口语练习优秀段落

英语口语练习优秀段落学习了多年的英语之后,学生只会读,并且写的能力有限。
他们无法用英语与别人交流。
下面是店铺带来的英语口语段落,欢迎阅读!英语口语段落精选Giving Life MeaningHave you thought about what you want people to say about you after you’re gone? Can you hear the voice saying, “He was a great man.” Or “She really will be missed.” What else do they say?One of the strangest phenomena of life is to engage in a work that will last long after death. Isn’t that a lot like investing all your money so that future generations can bare interest on it? Perhaps, yet if you look deep in your own heart, you’ll find something drives you to make this kind of contribution---something drives every human being to find a purpose that lives on after death.Do you hope to memorialize your name? Have a name that is whispered with reverent awe? Do you hope to have your face carved upon 50 ft of granite rock? Is the answer really that simple? Is the purpose of lifetime contribution an ego-driven desire for a mortal being to have an immortal name or is it something more?A child alive today will die tomorrow. A baby that had the potential to be the next Einstein will die from complication is at birth. The circumstances of life are not set in stone. We are not all meant to live li fe through to old age. We’ve grown to perceive life3 as a full cycle with a certain number of years in between. If all of those years aren’t lived out, it’s a tragedy. A tragedy because a human’s potential was never realized. A tragedy because a spark was snuffed out before it ever becamea flame.By virtue of inhabiting a body we accept these risks. We expose our mortal flesh to the laws of the physical environment around us. The trade off isn’t so bad when you think about it. The problem comes when we construct mortal fantasies of what life should be like. When life doesn’t conform to our fantasy we grow upset, frustrated, or depressed.We are alive; let us live. We have the ability to experience; let us experience. We have the ability to learn; let us learn. The meaning of life can be grasped in a moment. A moment so brief it often evades our perception.What meaning stands behind the dramatic unfolding of life? What single truth can we grasp and hang onto for dear life when all other truths around us seem to fade with time?These moments are strung together in a series we call events. These events are strung together in a series we call life. When we seize the moment and bend it according to our will, a will driven by the spirit deep inside us, then we have discovered the meaning of life, a meaning for us that shall go on long after we depart this Earth.给生命以意义你有没有想过,你希望人们在你死后怎样评论你?你能否听到这样的说,“他是个伟大的人”或“人们的确会怀念她”,他们还会说些什么?人生最奇异的现象之一就是,你从事的事业在你死后仍将长久存在。
你为什么不能打我英语作文

As a high school student, I have always been fascinated by the English language, and writing in English has become an integral part of my academic life. However, there was a time when I struggled with the idea of writing an essay in English, especially when it came to the topic of why someone might say, You cant hit me.This particular topic resonated with me because it brought to light the importance of communication and the power of words. Its not about physical violence, but rather about the emotional and psychological impact that words can have on a person. Its about standing up for oneself and asserting ones rights in a respectful and nonaggressive manner.Growing up, I was always taught to respect others and to avoid conflict whenever possible. But as I matured, I realized that sometimes standing up for oneself is necessary. Its about setting boundaries and not allowing others to take advantage of you. Its about being assertive and expressing oneself without resorting to aggression or violence.In my English class, we were assigned to write an essay on this topic. At first, I was hesitant. I didnt want to delve into such a sensitive and potentially controversial subject. But as I began to think more deeply about it, I realized that this was an opportunity to explore the complexities of human relationships and the importance of communication.I started my essay by discussing the concept of assertiveness and the difference between being assertive and being aggressive. I wrote about how assertiveness is about expressing ones thoughts and feelings in arespectful and confident manner, while aggression is about using force or intimidation to get ones way.I then went on to discuss the importance of setting boundaries and standing up for oneself. I wrote about how its okay to say no and to assert ones rights, even if it means facing potential backlash or criticism. I emphasized that its not about being confrontational or aggressive, but about being firm and respectful in ones communication.As I continued to write, I found myself drawing on personal experiences and observations. I wrote about times when I had to stand up for myself and assert my boundaries, and how it felt both empowering and challenging. I also wrote about the importance of empathy and understanding in these situations, and how its crucial to consider the feelings and perspectives of others.One of the most interesting parts of writing this essay was exploring the cultural and societal factors that influence our communication styles and attitudes towards conflict. I wrote about how different cultures have different norms and expectations when it comes to assertiveness and conflict resolution, and how this can impact our interactions with others.In conclusion, I wrote about the importance of finding a balance between standing up for oneself and being respectful towards others. I emphasized that its not about winning or losing, but about finding a way to communicate effectively and resolve conflicts in a healthy and constructive manner.Writing this essay was a challenging but rewarding experience. It forced me to confront my own beliefs and attitudes towards conflict and assertiveness, and it helped me to develop a deeper understanding of the complexities of human communication. It also reinforced my belief in the power of words and the importance of using them wisely and respectfully.In the end, the essay was wellreceived by my teacher and classmates, and it sparked a lot of interesting discussions and debates. But more importantly, it helped me to grow as a writer and as a person, and it reminded me of the power and potential of the English language to explore complex ideas and emotions.。
- 1、下载文档前请自行甄别文档内容的完整性,平台不提供额外的编辑、内容补充、找答案等附加服务。
- 2、"仅部分预览"的文档,不可在线预览部分如存在完整性等问题,可反馈申请退款(可完整预览的文档不适用该条件!)。
- 3、如文档侵犯您的权益,请联系客服反馈,我们会尽快为您处理(人工客服工作时间:9:00-18:30)。
What About Negation in Spatial Reasoning?Marco Ragni (Ragni@Informatik.Uni-Freiburg.de)Department of Computer Science, Georges-Köhler-Allee Geb. 52D-79110 Freiburg, GermanyThomas Fangmeier (Thomas.Fangmeier@Uniklinik-Freiburg.de) University Clinic Freiburg, Section for Experimental Neuropsychiatry, Hauptstraße 5D-79104 Freiburg, GermanyStefan Schleipen (Schleipe@Informatik.Uni-Freiburg.de)Department of Computer Science, Georges-Köhler-Allee Geb. 52D-79110 Freiburg, GermanyAbstractHow do we reason about spatial descriptions? In recent yearsa lot of research has been investigated in order to determine factors of complexity in spatial relational reasoning. Several factors like the number of models, the wording of conclusionor relational complexity have been determined. Any of these factors effected reasoning. But the literature focused solemnly on positive premises. Negated expressions like “The fork is not to the right of the cup” had not been investigated. Since in everyday spatial reasoning the role of negation is eminent, we study negated spatial relations from a formal and psychological perspective. Central questions are: How are negated statements represented? If there are various models consistent with the set of premises, which of these is constructed initially? Is there an effect for different reference frames? We conducted three experiments for investigating these research questions. We will show that humans (i) negatea relation by using the opposite relation, (ii) use a cognitive economic principle in constructing mental models, (iii) construct preferred mental models, and (iv) have more difficulties in reasoning with negated relations in comparisonto indeterminate positive descriptions.Keywords: Spatial Reasoning; Cognitive Modeling; Spatial Representation.IntroductionThere is a great body of evidence supporting the mental model theory of spatial reasoning. The key idea of this theory is that reasoners translate the spatial relations in the real or imagined world into a mental model and use this representation to solve given spatial inference problems. To provide an example (cf. Mani & Johnson-Laird 1982):(I) The plate is to the left of the knife.The fork is to the left of the knife.The glass is in front of the knife.The spoon is in front of the plate.This describes the following two possible models:spoon glass spoon glass fork plate knife plate fork knifeAssume a child helps his mother to set the table. The child takes the knife and puts it to the left of the plate. But the mother says to the child “The knife does not belong to the left of the plate”. Where will the child place the knife? Logically, there are three possibilities: the knife can be placed to the right of, in front of, or behind the plate (not considering that the knife can be placed above or under the plate). After a certain time the child will know how the single components should be placed on the table. This is what we call the background knowledge, but this goes beyond the scope of the paper.How are such problems with negation processed? Is there a preferred interpretation? The mental model theory (MMT), introduced by Johnson-Laird and Byrne (1991), suggests that people draw conclusions by constructing and inspecting a spatial array that represents the state of affairs described in the premises. It is a three stage process consisting of a comprehension, description, and validation phase. In the comprehension phase, reasoners construct a mental model that reflects the information from the premises. If new information is encountered during the reading of the premises it is immediately used in the construction of the model. During the description phase, this model is inspected to find new information that is not explicitly given in the premises. Finally, in the validation phase alternative models are searched that refute this putative conclusion. However, some questions remain open with regards to how people deal with multi-model problems. For example, which model is constructed first, and does this model construction adhere to certain principles? Why do reasoners neglect some models?All these questions are not answered by the classical mental model theory. In contrast, the preferred mental model theory (PMMT) has been developed to explain that humans generally tend to construct a preferred mental model (PMM). The PMM is the starting point for deriving at a putative conclusion. In the model variation phase the participants tend to make local and continuous transformations starting from the PMM to search counter-examples (Rauh et al., 2005).Several predictions of the PMMT about insertion principles as well as transformation strategies in spatialrelational reasoning can be shown (Ragni et al., 2006). Assume we have two premises of the form (1) “A is to the left of B” and (2) “A is to the left of C”. Humans tend to process these premises sequentially, i.e. first, a model A B is generated and then object C is inserted into the model. There are two possibilities where C can be inserted:•In between A and B (f irst f it principle, ff-principle) or •To the right of B (f irst f ree f it principle, fff-principle) PMMs are constructed by using the fff-principle. This has been empirically confirmed (Ragni et al., 2006).But how do humans process a premise like “A is not to the left of C”? Do they remain in one dimension (by using the opposite relation only)? Which kind of insertion principle is then used? Kaup and colleagues (2006) focused on the negation in sentences and contradictory predicates. They conducted a verification experiment in which participants had to verify sentences (e.g. the door was not open) and pictures of situations described in the sentences (e.g. closed door, open door). Reaction times were shorter if the sentence and the picture corresponded (affirmation). Since there are only two states possible (the door is open or the door is closed) there is only one opposite state left. Multiple model cases have not been investigated.Hasson and Glucksberg (2006) examined the difference in understanding affirmative and negated assertions in natural language. The participants had to make decisions on using terms related to either the affirmative or the negative meaning. The results suggest that the affirmative assertion continued to facilitate affirmative-related terms, but the negated assertion did not. In the literature, no work regarding negation in multiple model cases has been reported.In this paper, we analyze spatial problems with negated relations. The next section contains a formal analysis of negated spatial problems. In the following, we present empirical data in support of our theory. Finally, we discuss the results presented in the paper and give a short overview of some questions that are left open.Mathematical Background Johnson-Laird (2001) introduced the principle of truth: “A mental model can represent by default only what is true, but not what is false” (p. 434). The same property holds for mathematical models. It might be worth to analyze the formal structure of the problem: The processing of a premise is a function mapping of a linguistic statement consisting of a propositional statement like “A is to the left of B” to a position in a spatial array. This function is called an interpretation (Ebbinghaus, Flum, & Thomas, 1994). Mathematically it can be described as:I: Premise → Spatial ArrayFor the example introduced above “A is to the left of B”, an interpretation maps Object A to position (0,0) and object B to position (1,0) in a spatial array. But how is a negated statement “A is not left of B” interpreted? Answering this question depends on the used reference frame. Since we are using spatial relations, our reference frame in this case is a relational system consisting of already defined spatial relations. We only take the most parsimonious set of relations, i.e. the relations to the right of, to the left of, over and under. More complex relations then can be defined by using this set of relations and propositional connectives like logical ‘and’ and logical ‘or’. Take for instance the ternary relation “B is in between A and C”. This relation can be purely represented on a two-dimensional grid by using the four relations. Namely “B is in between A and C” is equivalent to all models of “A is left of B & B is to the leftof C or A is to the right of B & B is to the right of C or A is behind of B & B is behind of C or A is behind B & B is behind of C”.How can we define, based on the reference frame and the parsimonious relations, right, left, over, and under the negation of a relation? In our setting the following statementis always true: “A is left of B or A is to the right of B or Ais over of B or A is under B”, since there are no other possibilities (of course this could be easily extended to three-dimensions). If we receive a premise like “A is not tothe left of B”, then it follows logically that the rest of the sentence holds, i.e. that “A is to the right of B or A is overB or A is under B”. In several experiments the existence of preferred models has been confirmed (Rauh et al., 2005; Ragni et al., 2006). Compare the following two sets of premises(II) A is to the left of B. (III) A is to the left of B.C is to the right of A. C is not to the left of A.If we assume that participants interpret the relation “not” asthe opposite relation, then the premise sets II and III have identical models. Therefore, both kinds of problems lead to multiple model cases. There are several options to represent negation:•Reasoners could insert object A in an arbitrary position to B (arbitrary interpretation).•Reasoners could insert object A to the right of B with/without annotating other possibilities for A(opposite [annotated] interpretation).•Reasoners could insert object A to the left of B and annotating the violence on A(violating interpretation).Other interpretations are possible as well but these are the most reasonable. In the next section we provide data supporting the preferred annotated interpretation.Empirical DataWe report three experiments on how humans generate and inspect mental models out of given premises when a relationof a premise is negated.First, we questioned which relations between two objects were accepted if a relation was negated or not? Second, we were interested in the generation process of mental models: (i) How is a object inserted into a model if the relation ofthe object is negated? (ii) Do participants use certaininsertion principles if a model contains a negated relation in a given premise which leads to a preferred mental model during the construction process? (iii) Are the preferred mental models with negated problems different from indeterminate positive problems? Finally, we examined the constructed model during the inspection phase that participants had in mind: (i) Which influences have different construction directions, for example, if a model was built from left to right or from right to left, and different term arrangements in two-dimensional models? (ii) Are there differences between indeterminate and negated problems during the inspection phase?We assumed that the participants interpret the negation of the relation as the logical negation in the same dimension. According to this hypothesis, we expected for the premise “A is not to the left of B” that the participants construct a model in which “A is to the right of B”. Another assumption was that models with negated relations are harder to obtain than models without negation. A further assumption was that the complexity of the model that participants have in mind is higher if a relation is negated in comparison to an indeterminate relation.First Experiment - AcceptanceIn this experiment the participants had to accept or reject statements about positive or negated relations between two objects. In the first part of the experiment we tested the acceptance with an underlying grid, since the SRM use distinct positions in a grid for solid objects without overlapping. During the second part of the experiment we tested without an underlying grid as the grid could bias the acceptance due to a clear horizontal and vertical arrangement which is not natural for a mental model. Participants, Materials, Procedure and Design. Thirty-six students of the University of Freiburg took part in this experiment (with/without grid: n = 20/16, age in years: M= 24.3/24,SD = 2.4/2.8). The participants were presented with pictures of two related objects and a statement. Figure 1 shows examples with (I) and without (II) an underlying grid.Figure 1: I) with underlying grid, II) without underlying grid. Statement with negated relation of the two objects that have to be accepted or rejected.The letter A had a fixed position in the center while the letter B was randomly swapped through all other 48 free cells in the grid. Every possible constellation of A and B was presented with a statement (“B is not over A”, “B is not right of A”). We also asked for “B is over A”, and “B is right of A” in order to compare the data with positive cases. The last two statements were tested on 16 of the 48 possible cases (see Figure 2II). Response times and accuracy were recorded for each statement.Results. The participants made a clear decision for affirmative (right/over) and negated (not right/not over) statements. Figure 2 indicates that the distinction whether or not B is over/not over A is clear. In both cases (with or without underlying grid) the results are similar for all four statements (over/not over and right/not right).Figure 2: I) Contains the negated statement “not over”. The left square shows the overall decisions in percent for NO and the right square for YES answers (correct answers marked grey). The numbers below contain the reaction times for the correct decisions. II) Shows the affirmative statement “over” (all other information is similar to I).Reaction time for the negation problems with or without underlying grid for “not over” is significantly longer than for “not right” (with/without grid: t = 7.076/5.589, df = 19/15, p≤ 0.01), as well as affirmative problems for “over” in comparison to “right” (with/without grid: t = 3.326/4.062, df = 19/15, p≤ 0.01).In most cases the reaction time is significantly shorter (see Fig. 3) if the statement and the actual state of therelation of A and B is true (with/without grid: “not over ” t = 0.288/4.124, df = 19/15, p = n.s./p ≤ 0.01; “not right ” t = 1.717/3.186, df = 19/15, p = n.s./p ≤ 0.05; “over ” t = 2.810/3.550, df = 19/15, p ≤ 0.05/p ≤ 0.01; “right ” t = 4.157/2.422, df = 19/15, p ≤ 0.001/p ≤ 0.05).Figure 3: I) Contains the negated statement “not over”. The left square shows the overall reaction time for NO and the right square for YES answers (only correct answers without underlying grid). The numbers in parenthesis contain the reaction time with underlying grid. II) Shows the positive statement “over” (all other information is similar to I).Second Experiment - Simple generating experimentIn this experiment, we investigated how people constructa model if premises contain negated relations between twoobjects. Additionally, we analyzed if participants construct apreferred mental model (PMM). We assume an increase in the difficulty for the generation of negated problems due to the higher cognitive effort. Additionally, we test the accuracy of the participants with determinate problems,which serve as an exclusion criteria for the ability of the participants to deal with the relational reasoning problems. Participants, Materials, Procedure and Design. Twenty- three students of the University of Freiburg took part in this experiment (age in years: M = 25.81, SD = 4.45). It was designed as a pen and paper experiment consisting of sixteen problems (Table 1 and 2) in which the participant had to construct a mental model out of four given premises. This model should then be drawn on a sheet of paper. Themodels were varied in the dimension (one- and two-dimensional), determination (determinate and indeterminate) and negation (affirmative and negated). Every model was presented twice but had different term names (total of 16). All of the 16 problems were constructed in the same way. Four premises arranged five differentobjects with the relations left, right, over or under. Therelation of the third premise was always negated. Note thatmodels with negation were always indeterminate due to theundetermined position of the object.In order to guarantee that a model was constructed inworking memory only, each problem contained three pages.On the first page the participants were given the first two premises, the following two premises were given on the second page. The third page was empty and the participants were asked to draw only one model even if multiple models could be constructed. Additionally, the participants were instructed not to use any kind of aid (no sketch, etc).Table 1: Contains four premises for the positive I) and negative II) problems for one-dimensional (a, b) and two-dimensional (c, d) problems for both determinate (a, c) and indeterminate (b, d).Problem PMM/alternativemodels(1) A B C D E (a) A is to the left of B. B is to the left of C. I) C is to the left of D.II) C is not to the right of D. D is to the left of E. For negated problems:”C is placed right of D”is impossible (1) A B C D E (2) A B D E C (3) A B D C E (b) A is to the left of B. B is to the left of C.I) D is not to the left of B. II) D is to the right of B. D is to the left of E.For negated problems:”D is placed left of B”is impossible (1) A EB C D(c) A is over B.B is to the left of C.I) C is not to the right of D. II) C is to the left of D. D is under E.For negated problems:”C is placed right of D”is impossible (1) A E B C D (2) A EB DC (d) A is over B. B is to the left of C. I)D is not to the left of C. II) D is to the right of C. D is under E. For negated problems:”D is placed left of C” is impossibleResults. The correct answers (see Tab. 3) indicate, that one-dimensional problems are significantly more often correct than two-dimensional problems (Wilcoxon-Test: Z = 3.109, p = 0.002).Table 3: Shows the correct responses (in percent) forone- and two-dimensional, affirmative and negative, as well as determinate and indeterminate problems. Aff. = affirmation, Neg. = negation 1-dim. 2-dim.Aff. Neg. Aff. Neg.Det. 87 78 76 52 Indet.85 78 67 57Furthermore, there is a significant difference between affirmative and negated problems (Wilcoxon-Test: Z = 2.618, p = 0.009). However, there is no significant difference between determinate and indeterminate problems. An additional question was how participants understand negated problems. If one direction is negated, then all other possible directions are allowed. There was a stable preference for the opposite direction in negated problems. Table 4 shows that except for indeterminate two-dimensional problems the use of the opposite direction was significantly more frequent. A further question was the preference for a model. For both dimensions we found a significant difference from zero for the PMM. When we analyzed only the indeterminate problems for affirmative versus negated problems, we did not find any significant differences.Table 4: Shows the preference for the opposite direction in percent for one- and two-dimensional, as well as determinate and indeterminate negated problems. The numbers divided with colons denote the number of correct answers for the opposite direction in comparison to all correct answers. The last row indicates the proportion of preferred models (fff) in comparison to the other principle (ff). Note that the models for determinate negated problems in this task do not provide the discrimination between preferred and alternative models.*p ≤ 0.05, ** p ≤ 0.01, *** p ≤ 0.001NegationCorrect answers 1-dim. 2-dim. Det. Opposite 81%***83%**Opposite : all 29 : 36 20 : 24Indet. Opposite 75%*65%Opposite : all 27 : 36 17 : 26fff / ff 24*** / 3 14*** / 3Third Experiment - Model inspection and negationIn the last experiment we analyze the inspection phase forindeterminate and negated problems. We assume that ifparticipants were asked to inspect and validate recently constructed mental models the difficulty for negated problems increase due to the higher cognitive effort (determinate < indeterminate < negated problems).Participants, Materials, Procedure and Design. Sixteenstudents of the University of Freiburg (age in years: M =24.3, SD = 2.4) took part in this experiment. Two participants were excluded due to the low accuracy rate (≤ 50%) in determinate problems. We conducted a computer experiment in order to measure reaction time and accuracy as well as reading time for given premises. The experiment contained 24 problems, 12 one-dimensional and 12 two-dimensional (see Tab. 5). Table 5: Shows the material for the problems. The cursive part indicates the three different types of the models: negated (3a), indeterminate (3b), and determinate (3c). Half of the problems had the relation ”under” (I) in the fourth premise, the other “over” (II). Every type of problem was presented twice.Problem ExpectedPMM(a) 1 A is over B.2 B is to the left of C. 3a D is not to the left of B. 3b D is to the right of B. 3c C is to the left of D.4 D is under E. (I) / D is over E. (II) (I) A E B C D(II) A B C D E (b) 1 A is over B.2 B is to the right of C. 3a D is not to the right of B. 3b D is to the left of B. 3c C is to the right of D.4D is under E. (I) / D is over E. (II)(I) E A D C B(II) A D C B EAll four premises were given on the computer screen at once. After pressing a key the premises disappeared and a statement was presented. One object of the statement was taken from the third premise and the other object from another premise. This guaranteed that the participant had to prove the model containing the negated relation and had to infer an implicit relation between two objects. The relation in the statement was always missing so that the participant had to fill in the correct answer, or, in case of indetermination, a relation that seemed the most possible. There were four possible relations for an answer: left, right, over and under.Results. We found no differences in the overall premise reading times between determinate, indeterminate, and negated problems. The different term arrangements (introduced through the fourth premise with “under” or “over”) and dimension were not different likewise. Again, we found a strong preference for PMM (indeterminate/negated: alternative mental model, AMM = 24%; PMM = 76%; Binomial-Test p ≤ 0.001). The accuracy of the answers decreases significantly from determinate toindeterminate to negated problems (Page-L Test N = 14, k = 3, L = 178, p ≤ 0.05). General Discussion Without negated relations relational reasoning seems to be inherently incomplete. But how do humans interpret and reason with negated relations? A formal analysis showed that there are at least three possible interpretations. First of all, we are able to show that a negated expression like “A is not to the left of B” is interpreted by the opposite relation“to the right of”. Even, if the participants had the chance to use correctly “over” or “under” they tend to maintain thedimension (horizontal if the negated relation is horizontaletc.). Especially the independence of interpreting relations by using/not using a grid is remarkable. It is a justification for using grid structures as a help in both designing experiments and modeling relational reasoning computationally (SRM, Ragni et al., 2005) by using a grid. We found that the reaction time for the negation problems with or without underlying grid for “not over” was significantly longer than for “not right”. We assume that this depends on the dimension the participants had to reason about. It seems that it is more common for reasoners to handle horizontal tasks. Finally, negation is a form of reasoning with indeterminacy, but reasoning is more complex and leads to more errors than reasoning with determinate descriptions. The accuracy of the answers decreased from indeterminate positive relations to negated relations. Problems with negated premises offers a greater variety of consistent models, but as well as in the classical case (Ragni et al., 2006) humans tend to construct preferred mental models to reduce complexity. Predictions about how the negated relation is interpreted, as well as the construction of a preferred mental model could be confirmed by all experiments. In other words, there are definitively preferred mental models in reasoning with negated assertions, and in indeterminate cases the participants constructed the preferred models by using thefff-principle (Ragni, et al., 2006).Some previous research has covered the linguistic processing and comprehension. Kaup and colleagues (2006) showed that the processing of matching sentences and pictures are easier if the sentence and the picture correspond. Hasson and Glucksberg (2005) examined the question if negated information entails affirmation. They were able to show that negated metaphors are most likely represented as affirmation. However, in spatial reasoning multiple model cases are possible. Therefore, the negationof a spatial relation is not necessarily the opposite relation. Additionally, the information about other possible models has to be stored. In this case it seems reasonable to adapt an approach of Vandierendonck, Dierckx, and De Vooght (2004) for positive indeterminate model cases: to represent the alternatives by annotations at the object that is related with negation in the initial premises.Our investigation can be contrasted to the work of Gapp (1995) who investigated the question what kind of configuration of two objects could still be described by a relation. Participants had to rate configurations on a scale. As a result he showed that participants accepted for instance the relation “to the right of” in-between the angles ± 22.5°. Contrary, we did not offer a continuous scale, because humans have to build a mental model and therefore they usea unique interpretation.Negation plays an important role in representation and reasoning. Since negation makes reasoning difficult, even more than ambiguous descriptions, humans tend to construct preferred mental models. Future work should cover aspectsof how participants find or neglect counter-examples and how three-dimensional reference frames are being processed.AcknowledgmentsThis work was partially supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) as part of the Transregional Collaborative Research Center SFB/TR 8 Spatial Cognition. We like to thank Markus Knauff and Bernhard Nebel for various helps. We also owe thanks to three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.ReferenceEbbinghaus, H., D., Flum, J., Thomas, W. (1994). Mathematical Logic 2nd ed.. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Gapp, K. P. (1995). Angle, Distance, Shape and their Relationship to Projective Relations. In J. D. Moore & J. F. Lehman (Eds.). Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 112-117). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.Hasson, U., Glucksberg, S. (2006). Does understanding negation entail affirmation? An examination of negated metaphors. Journal of Pragmatics, in press.Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2001). Mental models and deduction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 434-442.Johnson-Laird, P. N., & Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Hove, UK: Erlbaum.Kaup, B., Luedtke, J., & Zwaan, R. A. (2006). Processing negated sentences with contradictory predicates: Is a door that is open mentally closed? Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1033-1050.Knauff, M., Rauh, R., & Schlieder, C. (1995). Preferred mental models in qualitative spatial reasoning: A cognitive assessment of Allen’s calculus. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 200-205). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Mani, K. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1982). The mental representation of spatial descriptions. Mem Cognit, 10 (2), 181-187Miller, G. A., & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1976). Language and Perception. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ragni, M., Fangmeier, T., Webber, L., & Knauff, M. (2006).Complexity in Spatial Reasoning. In Proceedings of the 28th Annual Cognitive Science Conference. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Ragni, M., Knauff, M., & Nebel, B. (2005). A Computational Model for Spatial Reasoning with Mental Models. In B. G.Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Cognitive Science Conference (pp.1064-1070). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Rauh, R., Hagen, C., Knauff, M., Kuß, T., Schlieder, C., & Strube, G. (2005). Preferred and alternative mental models in spatial reasoning. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 5, 239-269.Vandierendonck, A., Dierckx, V., & De Vooght G. D. (2004).Mental model construction in linear reasoning: Evidence for the construction of initial annotated models. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.57A, 1369-1391.。