Relationship of reading comprehension to the cognitive internal state lexicon
高中英语阅读理解解题技巧ppt课件

Main idea extraction method
Finding topic sentences
Common misconceptions and coping strategies
Misconception 1
Translate word by word and sentence by sentence
Response strategy
Pay attention to the connections between paragraphs, understand the overall structure and logical relationship of the article.
CATALOGUE
目 录
• Vocabulary and Phrase Accumulation and Application
• Improving Reading Speed and Standardizing Answering Questions
• Simulated test exercise and explanation
Response strategy
Learn to read meaning groups, understand the relationship between the main body and modifying elements of sentences, and grasp the central idea of the article.
The relationship between reading comprehension and critical thinking

The relationship between reading comprehension ORIGINAL ARTICLEand critical thinking:A theoretical studyAbdulmohsen S.AloqailiKing Saud University,College of Education,Department of Curriculum and Instruction,Saudi Arabia Received 11November 2010;accepted 11January 2011Available online 31October 2011KEYWORDSSchema theory;Reading comprehension;Critical thinking;Cognitive development processesAbstract The main purpose of the present study is to review and analyze the relationship between reading comprehension and critical thinking.The specific theatrical issues being discussed include schema theory as a rational premise for the connection between reading comprehension and critical thinking,cognitive development processes,critical thinking:its nature and definitions,critical thinking:skills and dispositions,and critical thinking and reading comprehension.The results revealed that:(1)there is well established relationship between reading comprehension and critical thinking,(2)schema theory provides a rational premise for that relation,and (3)there is no con-sensus regarding the definition of critical thinking which might be interpreted as a lack of an accepted framework for critical thinking.ª2011King Saud University.Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V.All rights reserved.1.IntroductionIn recent years,the field of research on reading comprehension and critical thinking has received much attention and became a popular area in cognitive psychology.Modern cognitivists have developed new trends and theories that provide theoretical models for explaining and conceptualizing reading comprehen-sion by utilizing a set of related concepts,such as criticalthinking,prior knowledge,inference-making,and metacogni-tive skills (Limbach and Waugh,2010;Zabit,2010).Among these trends is schema theory,which is considered to be a theory about knowledge:how knowledge is represented and or-ganized,and how that representation and organization facili-tates the use of a reader’s prior knowledge to improve reading comprehension.A schema is the organized knowledge that one has about people,objects,places,events,processes,concepts,and virtually everything that provides a basis for learning (Rumelhart,1984).Bos and Anders (1990)stated that,‘‘Schema theory explains how knowledge is structured in mem-ory and how these structures affect incoming information’’(p.49).Anderson and Wilson (1986)indicated that schema theory explains how people’s existing knowledge affects comprehension.Critical thinking can be considered as means to activate or construct schema.Norris and Phillips (1987)indicate that crit-ical thinking provides an explanation for activating existing schemata and for constructing new ones by contrasting ideas and engaging in reflective thinking.Moreover,Collins et al.E-mail address:aloqaili@.sa2210-8319ª2011King Saud University.Production and hosting by Elsevier B.V.All rights reserved.Peer review under responsibility of King Saud University.doi:10.1016/j.jksult.2011.01.001(1980)count inference-making as a way to activate schemata in terms offilling in the missing connections between the surface structure fragments of the text by recourse to content and knowledge about the world.McNeil(1992)asserts that schema theory has special rele-vance for teachers of reading comprehension in that it ques-tions the traditional view that students should learn to reproduce the statements being read in the text.In contrast to this older view of reading comprehension,schema theory stresses an interactive approach that views teaching reading comprehension as a process,meaning that students are taught techniques for processing text,such as making inference,acti-vating prior knowledge,and using critical thinking(McNeil, 1992;Aloqaili,2005a;Orbea and Villabeitia,2010).Tierney and Pearson(1986)explain that schema theory has the major influence on new views of reading and reading com-prehension.They stated that:New views have forced us to rethink the act of reading.Fora long time we thought reading was the reproduction of theideas on the page;our goal was to have students produce a ‘‘photocopy’’of the page.Schema theory has moved us away from a reproductive view to a constructive view.In that view,the reader,rather than the text moves to the cen-ter of the construction process(p.3).According to schema theory,there are no definitive orfinal conclusions that can be reached for the text(Norris and Phillips,1987;Yu-hui et al.,2010).That is,schema theory deals with the reading comprehension as an interactive process between readers’prior knowledge and the text being read. Sometimes a reader may end up with a different understand-ing,based on his or her total previous experiences:their rich-ness or paucity.Therefore a reader with a rich background will comprehend better than one who has a poorer back-ground.In short,schema theory believes in open text or con-text.The interpretation is relative(Aloqaili,2005b).For the purpose of the study,reading comprehension can be defined as the meaning constructed as a result of the complex and interactive processes relating a reader’s critical thinking,prior knowledge,and inference-making.2.Cognitive development processesPiaget(1952)presented three cognitive processes which he used to explain how and why cognitive/concept development occurs.These processes are assimilation,accommodation, and equilibrium.Marshall(1995)believes that Piaget made a key contribution to schema theory with his focus on how sche-mata develop and change.So,the following section will be devoted to a brief explanation of Piaget’s work related to cog-nitive development processes.Piaget(1970)proposed that cognitive growth occurs when the learner establishes mental categories(schemata)comprised of concepts about subjects and events sharing some general or spe-cific features.He views schemata as cognitive structures by which individuals intellectually adapt to and organize the environment.Piaget(1952)provides three cognitive mechanisms which interpret how children develop,acquire,classify,or organize their schemata or cognitive structures.These cognitive pro-cesses or mechanisms are assimilation,accommodation,and equilibration.2.1.AssimilationAccording to Piaget(1952)assimilation is a continuous process that helps the individual to integrate new,incoming stimuli into the existing schemata or concepts.That is,assimilation includes adding new information to old schemata.To illustrate the assimilation processes,Rubin(1997)presents an example of young children who tend to classify all similar four-footed animals as dogs;the children are assimilating.What they do is that they have assimilated all four-food animals into their existing schemata.Wadsworth(1996)points out that the assimilation process elaborates the size or growth of schemata,however,it does not alter them,and it is simply like adding air into a balloon.He stated the following:Assimilation theoretically does not result in a change of schemata,but it does affect the growth of schemata and is thus a part of development.One might compare a schema to a balloon and assimilation to putting more air in the bal-loon.The balloon gets larger(assimilation growth),but it does not change its shape.Assimilation is a part of the pro-cess by which the individual cognitively adapts to and orga-nizes the environment’’(p.17).Thus,assimilation allows for the growth of schemata by adding or taking in new information to old.However,the assimilation process does not change or create new schemata. Change and creation of schemata are the functions of another cognitive development process,which is accommodation.2.2.AccommodationPiaget(1952)indicates that accommodation is the process of developing new categories by a child rather than integrating them into existing ones.That is,accommodation is the way by which children create new schemata or change old ones with new information.Wadsworth(1996)explains that if the child meets with new stimulus that cannot be assimilated because there are no schemata into which the stimulus would fit,the alternative is either to construct new schemata in which to place the stimulus(a new index card in thefile),or change or modify the existing ones tofit with new stimulus.That is, accommodation has two aspects or forms:creation of new schemata or modification of old schemata with new ones.Wadsworth(1996)clarifies the difference between assimila-tion and accommodation by stating that‘‘accommodation accounts for development(a qualitative change)and assimila-tion accounts for growth(a quantitative change;together these processes account for intellectual adaptation and the develop-ment of intellectual structures’’(p.19).Rubin(1997)asserts that in spite of the importance of both assimilation and accommodation as a cognitive process devel-opment,children should be aware of making a balance between these two processes.Therefore,balancing between assimilation and accommodation is the function of the third cognitive mechanism,which is equilibrium.A brief explana-tion of equilibrium is provided below.2.3.EquilibriumAccording to Piaget(1952)equilibrium is a balance between the assimilation and accommodation processes.Wadsworth36 A.S.Aloqaili(1996)indicates that if a child overassimilates,he or she will end up with a few too large schemata,and will be unable to find out the differences in things,because most things seem similar to him or her.In contrast,if a child overaccommo-dates,he or she will have too many small schemata.This over-accommodation would prevent him or her from detecting similarities,because all things seem different to him or her.Rubin(1997)explains that a child with equilibrium process would be able to see similarities between stimuli and thus assimilate them,and also would be able to determine when new schemata are needed for adequate accommodation of a surplus of categories or schemata.3.Critical thinking:its nature and definitionsThe literature indicates that there is no consensus regarding the definition of critical thinking.A multiplicity and variation of definitions of critical thinking are reflective of the way in which educators and scholars define it(Aloqaili,2001;Minter, 2010).Romeo(2010)explains that there is currently a lack of an accepted framework for critical thinking,so that there is not a widely acknowledged and accepted theoretical definition. Some educators and psychologists deal with critical thinking as a narrow concept,whereas others view critical thinking as a broad concept.For example,Beyer(1987)defined critical thinking in a narrow sense as convergent thinking.He stated clearly that‘‘critical thinking is convergent’’(p.35),in contrast to creative thinking which is divergent.Beyer(1985)has argued that‘‘critical thinking is not a pro-cess at least not in the sense that problem solving or decision making are processes;that is,critical thinking is not a unified operation consisting of a number of operations through which one proceeds in sequence’’(p.303).Mcpeck(1981)has offered this broad definition for critical thinking,‘‘The propensity and skill to engage in an activity with reflective skepticism’’(p.8).Ennis(1993)criticizes Mcpeck’s definition because it focuses on‘‘reflective skepti-cism,’’and according to Ennis,‘‘critical thinking must get beyond skepticism’’(p.180).Ennis(1962)has dealt with crit-ical thinking with a narrow sense.He stated that critical think-ing is‘‘the correct assessing of statements’’(p.6).However, Ennis(1985)has replaced his narrower definition with the broader one which viewed critical thinking as‘‘reasonable, reflective thinking that is focused on deciding what to believe or do’’(p.46).One of the main differences between Ennis’definitions of critical thinking is that the broader definition includes creative elements,but the narrower one tried to exclude them.Ennis (1987)explains and analyzes his broader definition of critical thinking as follows:Critical thinking,as I think the term is generally used,is a practical reflective activity that has reasonable belief or action as its goal.There arefive key ideas here:practical,reflective, reasonable,belief,and action.They combine into the following working definition:Critical thinking is reasonable reflective thinking that is focused on deciding what to believe or do. Note that this definition does not exclude creative thinking. Formulating hypotheses,alternative ways of viewing a prob-lem,questions,possible solutions,and plans for investigating something are creative acts that come under this definition (p.10).Lewis and Smith(1993)indicate that although Ennis does not use the term problem solving in his definition of critical thinking,he refers to the usual steps in problem solving as cre-ative acts which are a part of his definition.In other words, Ennis separates critical thinking and problem solving while pointing out their interdependence in practice.Ennis(1987)explains that he abandoned his narrower def-inition of critical thinking‘‘because,although it provides more elegance in theorizing,it does not seem to be in accord with current usage’’(p.11).Kennedy et al.(1991)point out that current usage of the term‘‘critical thinking’’generally reflects Ennis’broad definition.According to Ennis(1993),for a person to reasonably and reflectively go about deciding what to believe or do,most of the following things characteristically must be done interdependently:Judge the credibility of sources.Identify conclusions,reasons,and assumptions.Judge the quality of an argument,including the acceptabil-ity of its reasons,assumptions,and evidence.Develop and defend a position on an issue.Ask appropriate clarifying questions.Plan experiments and judge experimental designs.Define terms in a way appropriate for the context.Be open-minded.Try to be well informed.Draw conclusions when warranted,but with caution.Another scholar who has provided a broad definition for critical thinking is Facione(1984)who developed a definition of critical thinking that incorporates evaluation and problem solving.Facione indicates that it is possible to evaluate critical thinking by evaluating the adequacy of the arguments that ex-press that thinking.He stated that‘‘critical thinking is the development and evaluation of arguments’’(p.259).Lewis and Smith(1993)point out that what is new in Facione’s definition is that he views critical thinking as an active process which involves constructing arguments,not just evaluating them.According to Facione(1984)construct-ing arguments include the usual steps of problem solving which are:(1)determining background knowledge,(2)gener-ating initially plausible hypotheses,(3)developing procedures to test these hypotheses,(4)articulating an argument from the results of these testing procedures,(5)evaluating the arguments,and(6),where appropriate,revising the initial hypotheses.Facione(1984)stated that‘‘Learning argument construc-tion means learning the methodologies that generations of researchers have refined for the specific needs of each disci-pline’’(p.259).In this study,critical thinking refers to the pro-cess by which the reader thinks reasonably and reflectively for the purpose of meaning construction.4.Critical thinking:skills and dispositionsThere is an argument between educators regarding whether critical thinking involves both skills and dispositions.If so, which skills and which dispositions?Skills(or abilities)are the more cognitive aspect of critical thinking,however,dispo-sitions(or attitudes)are the more affective aspect.The relationship between reading comprehension and critical thinking:A theoretical study37Beyer(1984)views critical thinking as a set of nine discrete skills,including:(1)distinguishing between verifiable facts and value claims,(2)determining the reliability of a source,(3) determining the factual accuracy of a statement,(4)distin-guishing relevant from irrelevant information,claims or rea-sons,(5)detecting bias,(6)identifying ambiguous or equivocal claims or arguments,(7)recognizing logical incon-sistencies or fallacies in a line of reasoning,(8)distinguishing between warranted or unwarranted claims,and(9)determin-ing the strength of an argument.A number of researchers in critical thinking disagree that critical thinking is only a set of skills,and they maintain that critical thinking also involves dispositions.So in the literature the importance of dispositions has been heavily stressed (Ennis,1987;Norris,1985;Baum and Newbill,2010;Facione, 2010;Zori et al.,2010;Sternberg,1985).Paul(1984)makes a useful distinction regarding the dispo-sitions of the thinker.He deals with critical thinking in two dif-ferent ways:critical thinking in the weak sense and critical thinking in the strong sense.He asserts:In a weak sense,critical thinking skills are understood as a set of discrete micrological skills ultimately extrinsic to the character of the person;skills that can be tacked onto other learning.In the strong sense,critical thinking skills are understood as a set of integrated macro-logical skills ultimately intrinsic to the character of the person and to insight into one’s own cognitive and affective processes (p.5).Paul(1991)indicates that critical thinking in the strong sense involves approaching issues from multiple perspectives and demands open-mindedness to understanding points of view with which one disagrees.Among those who advocated skills and dispositions were Ennis(1985),who defined critical thinking as‘‘reasonable, reflective thinking that is focused on deciding what to believe or do’’(p.46).Based on his broad and working definition of critical thinking,Ennis(1987)developed a taxonomy of critical thinking skills which includes thirteen dispositions and twelve abilities that together make up critical thinking.For example, some of the dispositions of a critical thinker,as mentioned by Ennis(1987)are:(1)Seek a clear statement of the thesis or question.(2)Take into account the total situation.(3)Keep in mind the original and/or basic concern.(4)Look for alternatives.(5)Use one’s critical thinking abilities.(6)Be sensitive to the feelings,level of knowledge,anddegree of sophistication of others.(7)Be open-minded.In addition to these dispositions,there are some abilities, such as:(1)focusing on a question,(2)analyzing arguments, (3)asking and answering questions of clarification and/or challenge,(4)judging the credibility of a source,(5)deducing and judging deductions,(6)inferring explanatory conclusions and hypotheses,and(7)identifying assumptions.Each of these abilities contains a large number of sub-abilities(Ennis, 1987).5.Critical thinking and reading comprehensionThe relationship between critical thinking and reading is well established in the literature.For example,Norris and Phillips (1987)point out that reading is more than just saying what is on the page;it is thinking.Moreover,Beck(1989)asserts ‘‘there is no reading without reasoning’’(p.677).Also,among those researchers and theoreticians who recognize that reading involves thinking is Ruggiero(1984).He indicates that reading is reasoning.Yu-hui et al.(2010)stated clearly that reading is a thinking process to construct meaning.Utilizing and combining schema theory with principles of critical thinking are one of the effective ways of enhancing the concept of reading comprehension(Norris and Phillips, 1987).They explain that critical thinking provides a means of explaining the ability to work out ambiguous text by gener-ating alternative interpretations,considering them in light of experience and world knowledge,suspending decision until further information is available,and accepting alternative explanations.They conclude that critical thinking is the pro-cess which the reader uses to comprehend.Schema theory provides powerful rationales for making links between students’individual backgrounds,specific sub-ject area knowledge,and critical thinking(Marzano et al., 1988;Aloqaili,2005c).According to Anderson(1994),there are six ways in which schemata function in thinking and in remembering text information.These six ways are:(1)Most new knowledge is gained by assimilating newinformation into existing structure;therefore,subject matter learning should build on prior knowledge when-ever possible.(2)The students’existing schemata help to allocate atten-tion by focusing on what is pertinent and important in newly presented materials.(3)Schemata allow and direct the inferential elaboration ofincoming information and experience.(4)Schemata allow orderly searches of memory by provid-ing learners with a guide to the types of information that should be recalled.(5)Schemata facilitate the thinking skills of summarizingand editing.(6)Schemata permit inferential reconstruction when thereare gaps in memory,which means that they help the learner generate hypotheses about missing information.It is obvious,based on the previous six schemata functions, that prior knowledge plays a significant role regarding estab-lishing connections between thinking critically and processing text information.This connection consequently leads the read-ers to reach the critical comprehension level.In accordance with this notion(the relationship between prior knowledge and critical thinking),the literature reveals an agreement between researchers concerning the idea that an individual’s familiarity with the subject matter of a text plays an important part in the person’s performance on think-ing tasks in that area(Glaser,1984;Norris,1985;Sternberg and Baron,1985).Knowledge and thinking skills can be viewed as interdependent(Nickerson et al.,1985).Comprehension itself has been seen as a critical thinking process.For instance,from a schema theory description of38 A.S.Aloqailireading,comprehension can be conceptualized as a critical thinking act(Anderson and Pearson,1984;Collins et al., 1980;Norris and Phillips,1987;Rumelhart,1980;Aloaili, 2005d).Lewis(1991)argues that viewing reading as a critical thinking act becomes more tenable when some of the compo-nents of the reading process are accepted as automatic and necessary(automatic processes like word identification,deriva-tion of meaning for most words,and assignment of impor-tance),but not sufficient for constructing text understanding.According to schema theory,the understanding and inter-pretation of the text are relative,which means that definitive conclusions cannot be reached.However,the readers should seek to arrive at a coherent and consistent understanding of the text being read.Lewis(1991)stated the following: Schema theory posits that there is no absolute meaning on the page to be interpreted the same by all-that is,there is no ‘‘correct’’comprehension.The goal of reading extended text is to arrive at a coherent representation of the text.This goal is achieved by readers’weighing and comparing data from their schemata,the text,and the context in which the act occurs(p.421).In order to enhance readers’ability to achieve and practice comprehension as a critical thinking act,researchers have shown that‘‘the critical thinker uses his or her metacognitive knowledge and applies metacognitive strategies in a planful, purposeful way throughout the critical thinking process’’(French and Rhoder,1992,p.191).Gallo,1987)uses metacognitive strategies to develop criti-cal thinking.She suggests that improved critical thinking re-quires developing the processes of observation,analysis, inference,and evaluation.Broek and Kremer(2000)made connections between infer-ence-making and critical thinking to promote reading compre-hension.They presented the idea that inferential and reasoning skills are closely related to other readers’characteristics and skills that affect text comprehension.Broek and Kremer (2000)state that:To be successful,readers must have the inferential and rea-soning skills to establish meaningful connections between information in the text and relevant background knowledge. Central to these skills is knowing what constitutes an inferen-tial or causal/logical relation and being able to recognize or construct one when needed in order to form a coherent mental representation of the text(pp.11–12).Ennis(1987)classified inference as critical thinking ability which includes three somewhat overlapping and interdepen-dent kinds of inference:deductive inference,inductive infer-ence,and inference to value judgments.According to Albrecht(1980),deduction is referred to as‘‘top-down think-ing’’because the conclusion or result is known and the search is for specific evidence that led to that particular conclusion. However,Clarke,1990pointed out that induction is often called‘‘bottom-up thinking’’because conclusions are drawn from specific instances,such as building on another unit the conclusion is reachedEnnis(1987)presented subskills or abilities under each of these three kinds of inference:deductive inference,inductive inference,and inference to value judgments.For example, deductive inference includes(1)class logic,(2)conditional logic,and(3)interpretations of statements.Also,inductive inference involves(1)generalizing,(2)inferring explanatory conclusions and hypotheses,and(3)giving reasonable assumptions.Moreover,inference to value judgments requires (1)background facts,(2)considering alternatives,and(3)bal-ancing,weighing,and deciding.Bizar and Hyde(1989)argued that inferential thinking con-tains two types:drawing inferences and drawing conclusion. Regarding thefirst one(drawing inferences),Bizar and Hyde (1989)stated the following:Inferential thinking involves putting together individual bits of information to derive a greater meaning than what one might expect from merely focusing on the bits themselves. When reading a passage,we infer a great deal;that is,we derive much more meaning than a literal interpretation of words’’(p.35).Another kind of inferential thinking,drawing a conclusion, involves taking pieces of information and synthesizing them into a meaningful idea which is greater than the separate pieces (Bizar and Hyde,1989).They concluded that drawing infer-ences and conclusions depend heavily on students’schemata. That is,if the student does not have the requisite knowledge or accurate schemata,he or she will not be able to build mean-ing from the materials being read.6.ConclusionThe literature reveals an agreement between theorists and researchers that there is a strong relationship among reading comprehension,critical thinking,and prior knowledge.This relation is interdependent,which means that prior knowledge serves as a foundation for critical thinking and inference-mak-ing.Critical thinking and inference-making work as effective means to activate prior knowledge.Prior knowledge and thinking skills can be viewed as interdependent.Schema theory provides powerful rational and theoretical premises of building an interactive model for interpreting how reading comprehen-sion develops by utilizing the connections between reading comprehension and critical thinking.Schema theory is consid-ered to be one of the most effective current theories that has had a major influence in terms of changing the face of reading instruction and reading comprehension.ReferencesAlbrecht,K.,1980.Brain Power:Learn to Improve Your Thinking Skills.Englewood Cliffs,Prentice Hall,NJ.Aloqaili,A.S.,2001.Perceptions of Saudi Arabian reading teachers of selected concepts related to schema theory.Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation,Ohio University,USA.Aloqaili,A.S.,2005a.An evaluation of Arabic teachers’education program in Teachers’Colleges in Saudi Arabia.Journal of the Faculty of Education29(4),301–382.Aloqaili,A.S.,2005b.Toward a modern standard of reading skills for elementary schools.Journal of Reading and Literacy49,77–146. Aloqaili,A.S.,2005c.The role of school in reading development of young learners.Paper presented at the International Conference Titled(Toward a Literate Arabic Society:Reading Supporting Policies:International Experiences),Casablanca,Morocco. Aloaili,A.S.,2005d.Arabic teachers beliefs and practices in Riyadh city in relation to constructivism.The Educational Journal19(76), 253–310.Anderson,R.C.,1994.Role of the reader’s schema in comprehension, learning,and memory.In:Ruddell,R.B.,Ruddell,M.R.,Singer,H.(Eds.),Theoretical Models and Processes of Reading,fourth ed.International Reading Association,Newark,DE,pp.469–82.The relationship between reading comprehension and critical thinking:A theoretical study39。
读后感有哪些短句怎么写的好

读后感有哪些短句怎么写的好在学术写作中,读后感是学生所需完成的主要作业之一。
一个好的读后感应该清晰地反映学生的思考和理解,并能使用专业术语来描述书中的重要内容。
以下是一些关于写作好的读后感的技巧,以及一个具体的例子来帮助理解。
第一,要对读的书进行概括。
在这个部分,作者应该简单地描述书的主要内容,重点和主题。
简单明了的概括准确地表述了读者对书的理解,让读者清晰地知晓作者的论述方向。
同时,它也为后面更深入、更具体的论述打下基础。
第二,要使用具体的例子来支撑自己的读后感。
在这个部分,作者应该将他所述的主题和课程材料互相连系。
举例来说,如果涉及到人类心理学相关的主题,作者可以引用材料来强调人类心理方面的相关讨论。
这样不仅是为了支持自己的观点,更是为了提高文章的可信度。
只有专业性、有力地支撑观点,文章才更易于引发读者的兴趣,并且对读者产生积极的影响。
第三,要总结并引入个人想法。
在这个部分,作者应对整篇文章进行总结和回应,并进一步展示自己对材料和主题的理解和思考。
这部分的写作应该是清晰的、明确的,强调作者的想法和反应,同时也应该保证严谨。
在这个阶段,作者可以提出一些自己的建议,以期针对到该话题进一步探讨,从而产生更有价值的想法。
以下是一个700字的读后感例子,以具体说明上述三个方面的写作要点。
阅读理解模型及其相关研究Books such as "Reading comprehension models" not only explore the theoretical and practical aspects of reading comprehension models, but also their relationship with other skills like decoding, listening, and comprehension. In his book, author David Pearson provides a comprehensive overview of the subject matter, including discussion of the models’ theoretical foundations, their research results, and the practical implications of their findings.The book uses case studies, teacher reflections, and classroom experience data to link the models discussed with real-world implications. One example from the book of particular interest is the work of M. Louise Green. Green’s research delves into differences in vocabulary and comprehension skills between groups of students from different socioeconomic backgrounds.As a reader, the goal in reading this book is twofold: First, to process and understand the material to enable self-growth and development, and secondly, to use this material to identify how it applies to the practical world-- both within the classroom and beyond.By framing the models in the context of practical implications, this book demonstrates the practical relevance of reading comprehension models. While the research and theoretical aspects covered in the book are of interest to theorists, teachers, educational administrators and policymakers will benefit from its insights into how these models can be implemented to enhancepractical outcomes.Overall, Pearson’s book provides valuable theoretical understandings and practical ideas that can assist teachers in enhancing their students’ reading comprehension abilities. By prompting further reflection and conversation by presenting a range of viewpoints, Pearson demonstrates the importance and utility of understanding reading comprehension models for further research.总之,读后感是学生在学术写作中需要经常完成的一项任务。
2024年度English Reading comprehension problem-solvin

01
02
03
04
05
Literal
Inference
comprehension
Vocabulary in context
Main idea a…
Author's pu…
Questions that ask for direct information from the text
Questions that require readers to infer implied means or draw conclusions based on the text
2024/3/23
6. Review and refine
Check your answer for accuracy and completeness, ensuring it fully addresses the question's requirements
7
02
Analysis of article structure and logical
Identify the subject, verb, main idea of
complex senses
Recognize and understand various sensitivity structures, including compound, complex, and compound compound senses
differences in meaning
Utilize context includes to infer the meaning of unknown words
2024/3/23
13
科技英语阅读理解

Summarizing the main ideas and taking notes while reading will help in retaining information and deepen understanding.
Expand Vocabulary
Overview of Reading Comprehension in Science and Technology English
01
Technical Terminology
Scientific English uses a large amount of technical and specialized terminals, making it challenging for non experts to understand
Selection of reading materials
Key information should be extracted from the text, including the main idea, supporting details, and the conclusion
Difficult words and phrases should be looked up in dictionaries or glossaries to enhance comprehension
1
2
3
Regular practice and training are essential to improve reading speed and accuracy
01
02
03
Reading and writing relations

Reading and writing relations: Second language perspectives on research and practiceBy William GrabeIn the past two decades, the role of reading-writing specialists. Interaction between reading and writing is thus a topic of concern in the academy as it relates to students studying in their first language (L1) as well as students studying in their second language (L2). In L1 contexts, a number of seminal studies on reading-writing relations appeared in the 1980s. In L2 contexts, the study of reading-writing relationships has evolved more slowly. L2researchers pointed out that cultural and language differences among L2 students create complexities cultural socializations and belief systems.A recent collection of papers provides numerous individual perspectives on the topic. I should comment on the relative roles of reading and writing as seen separately in research on reading-writing relations. This relationship is most commonly discussed in terms of the impact of reading on writing, the uses of readings to help students carry out writing tasks, and the ways in which student learning from texts is reflected in writing tasks.Theories of reading, writing, and reading-writing relationsReading-writing relations should then build on these theories and offer a framework that explains the ways in which reading and writing together enhance language, literacy, and content learning. The ability to read is typically assumed in many discussions of reading-writing relations. However, even in L1 settings, limited reading abilities sometimes make it difficult for students to carry out tasks that combine reading and writing. L2 readers may have difficulties writing from textual sources simply because of weaker language skills and reading comprehension abilities. The processing and problem-solving components of writing development can make intense demands on students, particularly when students are reading difficult L2 texts in order to collect or glean new information for their writing. In many reading-writing tasks, students are forced to make a number of complex decisions. Students need to decide the following:1.How much information should be taken from the text; which information shouldbe taken2.How the information taken will fit with task and writer goals3.How accurately the information should be represented when going from textsource to student writing4.What formal mechanisms should be used for transforming or using the textualinformationWriting from multiple texts requires even more demanding planning, processing, and revising. The interpretation of task demands and the integration of textual information force critical decision making that requires much practice and consistent efforts to “traverse the topical landscape” from multiple directions.Reading and writing together: L1 researchReading-writing relations have been a topic of L1 research for the past two decades. This early research base has also argued that reading and writing together can lead to effective learning. A set of articles by shanahan and Tierney (1990; Shanahan, 1990; Tierney & Shanahan, 1991) summarize the 1980s decade of work and three fundamental directions in L1 research on reading-writing relations: (1) shared processing and knowledge resources in reading and writing, (2) reading and writing as interaction, and (3) reading and writing to learn content. First, the goal of “shared processing” examines the overlap of processing skills. Students are tested in various reading abilities and writing abilities, and then they are compared on these abilities. It reveals considerable room for differences between reading and writing. Second, “reading and writing as interaction” highlights the notion of dialogue at a distance through the medium of the written text. Third, “learning content from reading and writing together” has been less thoroughly demonstrated. The L2 work on reading-writing relationships has continued under two major organizing issues: (1) reading to write, and (2) writing to learn from multiple texts.In the area of reading to write, three issues play major roles: (1) the continuing observation that better readers tend to be better writers across continuing observation that better readers tend to be better writers across a range of writing tasks; (2) the argument that recognizing and using the organization framework of the text leads to better writing over time. Readers are, in general, better able to collect, organize, and connect information in their writing. Work on writing from multiple texts also suggests two interesting lines of research: (1) the cognitive processing required for such tasks (and the resultant learning) (2)the interaction of reading and writing as tasks are carried out. Recent research on reading-writing interactions has demonstrated that students use the first reading to form a framework for summarizing when working with multiple texts, and they add relatively few main ideas after the second text have demonstrated that expert readers in a discipline integrate and use multiple texts in very different ways from more novice students when composing an argument.Reading and writing together: L2 researchFurther topics have also been sources of continued discussions. One is the argument that extensive reading directly improves writing abilities (Krashen, 1984, 1993). The second involves the role of directionality between reading and writing. (Is it better to go from reading to writing or from writing to reading for the most effective instruction?) There is also evidence that L2 literacy abilities can transfer back to a later developing L1 literacy (Wagner, 1998). There appears to be considerable evidence that certain types of literacy skills transfer to support L2 literacy development. Hypothesis involves similarities in writing processes and strategies based on small groups of students engaged in think-aloud tasks, found conflicting patterns of L1 influences on L2 performance.The Language Threshold HypothesisThis hypothesis argued that students must develop a reasonable L2 language proficiency before they will transfer L1 reading processes and strategies. Is L2 reading more dependent on L2 proficiency or L1 reading skills? The Language Threshold Hypothesis could say that the relationship between L2 proficiency and L2 reading abilities is about twice as strong as the relationship between L1 reading abilities and L2 reading abilities. It is also important to note that the concept of a language threshold is reading-writing relations. Considerable extensive reading, will lead to better writing abilities. In parallel with the L2 perspectives, L1-based research on exposure to print has argued that extensive reading leads to language knowledge that supports better writing abilities. For example, research has shown that extensive reading leads to better vocabulary knowledge, better verbal fluency, better syntactic knowledge, better semantic memory, better metalinguistic awareness, and broader knowledge of the world (see Stanovich et al., 1996; Wagner & Stanovich, 1996) It would be possible to make claims about the impact of extensive reading on literacy development equally as strong as those that can now be made for the Language Threshold Hypothesis.The directionality issueOne early research area that drew more specifically on issues of reading-writing relations examined directionality; that is, either (1) reading improves writing, or (2) writing improves reading, or (3) reading and writing improve each other (4) there is no direct relationship. One cannot assume that a good reader is a good writer.Further research issuesFirst, summarizing has been relatively neglected as a research area as applied toreading to write, writing to learn, and writing from multiple source texts,reading-writing relations in EAP contexts strongly suggest that summarization is a major issue for literacy development and content learning. The role of cross-cultural expectations and writing performance. Particularly for L2 writing, the notion of contrastive rhetoric needs to be incorporated more fully into discussions ofreading-writing relations. Curricula for language and literacy learning needed to ensure effective instruction; and appropriate means to assess effectiveness of learning and make adjustments as needed. The two frameworks that offer the best opportunities to incorporate reading and writing within realistic academic demands are content-based instruction and task-based instruction. Content-based instruction offers an affective way to provide a coherent set of reading and writing experience, and it can provide much practice in the specific literacy tasks expected in advanced academic settings. Frameworks for theme-based instruction that can support reading-writing interaction content-based learning, assumes advanced academic contexts and tasks that work well to support reading-writing integration. Task-based learning focus on lower levels of language learning and do not propose intensive reading-writing interactions for advanced academic purposes.Instructional practices in L2 contexts1. Reading and writing instruction should begin from task analyses. This perspective is the complement to creating effective prompts for writing.2. Students need to practice writing many types of relevant genres and tasks.3. Students need to develop rhetorical stances to tasks and texts that will build reading-writing relations.4. Students need to develop an awareness of text structure itself.5. Helping students become strategic readers and strategic writers should be a major goal for any EAP curriculum.6. Helping students to collect and use feed back from peers and teachers.7. Teaching students to collect and interpret data on interesting issues and topics.8. A fundamental goal for an EAP curriculum should be to use textual resources appropriately in writing takes. Aside from formatting, quoting, and plagiarism issues that are commonly noted in discussions of writing from resources, other important skills are needed for determining the strength of arguments, inferring the author’s position, incorporating strong examples and illustrations, making effective links between ideas across texts, and presenting critical comparisons of conflicting information. The development of these abilities requires a considerable commitment to writing from texts. Part of the difficulties that students have with these skills is that they are not asked to practice them on a regular basis. Without consistent efforts towork out effective plans for writing from texts, they can not be expected to achieve success regularly. However, being able to write effectively from texts is a major expectation in many academic settings.9. An important early step for students to take in learning to work from texts is being able to summarize main ideas from a text and synthesize main information across two or more texts. These skills can be developed initially from fairly simple tasks and can be practiced enough times in varying settings to allow for the development of effective practices and problem-solving routines. They allow for reasonably straightforward feedback and support, and these skills are essential aspects of larger tasks requiring writing from text resources.10. Finally, any reading and writing curriculum will need to incorporate effective ongoing assessment practices for reading and writing integration.。
新概念英语第一册第七课

To be able to understand and engage in simple shopping conversations, and to express shopping needs and intentions in English.
Emotional goal
What is she doing? (What is she doing?)
Analysis
This sentence is a declarative sentence in the simple present tense, indicating a frequent behavior.
Imitation
Associative memory method
associating new words with known things or images to form interesting associations for memory. For example, when memorizing an elephant, one can imagine an elephant with a long water pipe coiled around its trunk.
01
02
03
04
Vocabulary 2: Important
Part of speech: adjective
Meaning: Important
Example sentence: It's important to drive on time (It is important to arrive on
Vocabulary Expansion and Memory Skills
阅读表达的英语作文

阅读表达的英语作文Title: The Art of Reading Comprehension。
Reading comprehension is a vital skill that not only enhances one's understanding of various subjects but also enriches one's language proficiency. It requires a combination of critical thinking, linguistic ability, and analytical skills. In this essay, I will delve into the strategies and techniques that can help improve one's reading comprehension abilities.First and foremost, it is essential to develop good reading habits. Reading regularly exposes individuals to diverse writing styles, vocabulary, and ideas. Whether it's fiction, non-fiction, newspapers, or academic articles, each type of reading material offers its own set of challenges and benefits. By engaging with a wide range of texts, readers can sharpen their comprehension skills and become more adept at understanding complex ideas.Active engagement with the text is another crucial aspect of improving reading comprehension. This involves techniques such as annotating, highlighting key points, and asking questions while reading. By interacting with the material in this way, readers can better retain information and make connections between different concepts. Additionally, summarizing passages in your own words can help solidify understanding and identify the main ideas.Moreover, understanding the structure of a text can significantly aid comprehension. Being able to recognize the organization of a passage, including the introduction, body paragraphs, and conclusion, can provide valuable context for understanding the author's main argument or message. Furthermore, paying attention to transitional words and phrases can help follow the flow of ideas and understand the relationship between different parts of the text.Vocabulary plays a crucial role in comprehension as well. A strong vocabulary enables readers to grasp the nuances of language and accurately interpret the meaning ofwords in context. Building vocabulary can be achieved through various methods such as reading extensively, keeping a vocabulary journal, and actively learning new words through flashcards or online resources.Additionally, understanding the author's purpose and perspective is essential for effective comprehension. By considering factors such as the author's background, audience, and rhetorical strategies, readers can gain insight into why certain arguments are presented and how they are intended to be interpreted. This critical analysis allows readers to evaluate the credibility of the information presented and form their own informed opinions.Furthermore, practicing different types of reading comprehension questions can help develop specific skills, such as identifying main ideas, making inferences, and analyzing arguments. By familiarizing oneself with common question formats and strategies for approaching them, readers can feel more confident and prepared when encountering similar questions in future texts or exams.In conclusion, improving reading comprehension is a multifaceted process that requires dedication, practice, and critical thinking. By cultivating good reading habits, actively engaging with the text, understanding text structure, expanding vocabulary, considering the author's purpose, and practicing various comprehension skills, individuals can enhance their ability to understand and interpret written material effectively. Ultimately, mastering reading comprehension not only benefits academic success but also fosters a deeper appreciation and understanding of the world around us.。
- 1、下载文档前请自行甄别文档内容的完整性,平台不提供额外的编辑、内容补充、找答案等附加服务。
- 2、"仅部分预览"的文档,不可在线预览部分如存在完整性等问题,可反馈申请退款(可完整预览的文档不适用该条件!)。
- 3、如文档侵犯您的权益,请联系客服反馈,我们会尽快为您处理(人工客服工作时间:9:00-18:30)。
WARNINGConcerning Copyright RestrictionsThe copyright law of the United States (Title 1.7, United States Code) governs the making of photocopies or other reproductions of copyrighted material.Under certain conditions specified in the law, libraries and archives are authorized to furnish a photocopy or reproduction.One of three specified conditions is that the photocopy or reproduction is not to be used for any purpose other than private study, scholarship or research.If electronic transmission of reserve material is used for purposes in excess of what constitutes “fair use”, that user may be liable for copyright infringement.This policy is in effect for the following document:Relationship of Reading Comprehension to the Cognitive Internal State Lexicon.Booth, James and Hall, William.SOURCE:Reading Research Report Spring 1994 No.14: 1-35.NRRCNational Reading Research Center Relationship of Reading Comprehensionto the Cognitive Internal State LexiconJames R. BoothWilliam S. HallUniversity of Maryland College ParkREADING RESEARCH REPORT NO. 14Spring 1994The work reported herein is a National Reading Research Project of the University of Georgiaand University of Maryland. It was supported under the Educational Research and Development Centers Program (PR/AWARD NO.was a cognate of think or know.Cognitive words such as think and know form a category within the internal state lexicon(HallHall, 1991). Many cognitive words are polysemous and can be de-fined along a hierarchy from simpleperception to complex planning(FrankHughes, 1987). According to this hierarchy, the higher the level of mean-ing- the more conceptually demanding- the more internal processing is required. We argue that cognitive words may be centrally involved in the development of skilled reading compre-hension.Cognitive words can provide a medium that makes it possible to engage in metacognitive acts relevant to the reading process, such as generating a goal for reading, communicating the intended meaning of a text, and evaluating one’s level of understanding. Similarly, cogni-tive words can equip the reader with a vehicle by which to evaluate different comprehension strategies critically or to reflect on the logicalorganization and interdependence of the2James R. Booth & William S. Hall ponents of a text. Our elaborated cognitiveword lexicon allows us to make fine-graineddistinctions between cognitive states. Cognitivewords “convey shades of meaning which addsuccinctness and precision to the lexicon” andsupply us with “a greater capacity for descrip-tion and definition” (Corson, 1985, p. 61).While skilled reading comprehension requiresthe use of all of the aforementioned skills,direct empirical evidence for the claim thatcognitive word knowledge is central to thedevelopment of reading comprehension issparse (see OlsonBeeghly,and Silber (1983)found that the earliest use of cognitive words isfor pragmatic or conversational purposes, suchas in directing the action. By the end of theirthird year, children begin to use cognitivewords in a way that suggests semanticbut they do notunderstand the distinctions between manycognitive words, such as remember, know, andguess, until approximately four years of age(Johnson 1980). At around fiveyears of age, children can differentiate betweenknow and think (JohnsonFurrow,Green-berg, 1978; Moore et al.of the verb (Olson& Torrance, 1986); the think and guess dis-tinction is not attained until children are eightyears old (Moore et al., 1989). Furthermore,children do not understand believe, which canbe both until after ageseven (Abbeduto andeven high school and college students haveincomplete knowledge of more complex cogni-tive words such as predict, interpret, infer,conclude, and assume(AstingtonOlson, 1976); see and know refer to internalversus external experienceReading Comprehension and Cognitive Words3 1987). The development of the internal statelexicon is a gradual, incremental process thatdepends on the particular cognitive words to belearned. An example of this dependence is thatmany cognitive words are polysemous, that is,they have more than one meaning. Thiscognition, and planning were not. Hughes(1985) obtained similar results in a compre-hension task assessing cognitive word knowl-edge in three-, six-, and nine-year-old children.Frank and Hall (1991) then tried to incor-porate the cognitive word think into their levelof meaning hierarchy. They found that evalua-tion was used most often and that the other fivelevels of meaning were statistically undifferen-tiated for both children and adults. On the basisof these findings, they proposed that cognitivewords are organized hierarchically, but thatdifferent cognitive words may have a differentorganization of levels that depends on bothconceptual difficulty and prototypicality. Ac-cording to their hypothesis, prototypical mean-ings will be acquired first, but cognitive wordswhose prototypical meanings are of a lowerlevel will be semantically mastered earlier thanwords whose prototypical meanings are of ahigher level. This claim is supported byLinn, 1984).Know may have a lower level prototypicalmeaning than think.Certain cognitive words have several prag-matic as well as semantic functions, and thismay encourage children to developIn contrast, aREADING RESEARCH CENTER, READING RESEARCH REPORT NO. 144James R. Boothuse contributes indirectly, if at all, to the meaning of an utterance; it might be a hedge, a conversational device, an indirect request, or an attention-getting device such as “You know, I need to go to the store” (Hallguistic abilities. For example, when a childrealizes that a word is only a symbol for its referent, that context determines themeaning acqui-sition also depends on nonlinguistic strategies.Nonlinguistic strategies refer to a particular conceptual organization possessed by a child that coincides with and therefore makes the acquisition of certain word meanings easier (see Clark, 1980). For example, Clark (1973) claims that children acquire locative (in, on, and under) terms in a regular developmental sequence. Consequently, children may acquire certain cognitive words comparatively late because of their own conceptual organization. Objects, situations, and relations are directly observable; therefore, acquisition of words referring to these concepts is probably influ-enced directly by nonlinguistic strategies. In contrast, most cognitive words are verbs, and verbs have a more abstract and indistinct mental representation than nouns (seeReading Comprehension and Cognitive Words 5states; therefore, it is unlikely that cognitive words are mapped onto pre-existing conceptual categories. Instead, language (cognitive words)probably influences the development of chil-dren’s concepts of complex mental states invery important ways (ScholnickMeux, 1970) or written byparents or teachers (Carroll et al., 1971;Thorndikeexplain,hypothesize, infer, and interpret are absent, and believe occurs only once. This deficiency is particularly disturbing since many text-based academic skills require the cognitive monitor-ing that cognitive words label (Hall et al.,1987). “What schooling appears to provide is competence in talking about text, about ques-tions, about answers, in a word, competencewith a metalanguage” (Olson563).Research suggests that the cognitive word lexicon may develop late in children because the necessary linguistic input is deficient. The role of linguistic input in the development of cognitive word knowledge is uncertain; only two studies have compared children’s and adults’ use of cognitive words. The few studies that have been conducted have found a signifi-cant relationship. For example, in a study of olds, Beeghly, Bretherton, and Mervis (1986) found that the numbers and kinds of internal state utterances by the mother were positively correlated with the child’s spoken frequency of different internal state words, of internal state words referring to self and other, and of decontextualized internal state words. Similarly, Hall et al. (1987) found a significant correlation between child use (4years 6 months to 5 years) and parental use in both levels of meaning and the diversity of cognitive words used. These studies suggest that a child’s development of cognitive word knowledge is highly dependent on adult fre-quency of verbal use.Several studies also suggest that exposure to text may facilitate cognitive word learning.The different contexts in which a cognitive word appears in text may refine its definitionsand functions (OlsonOlson, 1983; Olson level text understanding.Despite the research suggesting a strongrelationship between cognitive words andNO. 146James R. BoothTorrance, 1986, 1987; Astington graders but not for first-graders. In another experiment, Olson and Torrance (1987) found cognitive word knowledge in third-graders to be significantly correlated with a listening com-prehension score that measured inferences drawn from the story. In addition, Olson andTorrancea key element in text understanding. Furthermore, knowledge of cognitive words seems to be centrally involved in reading comprehension. For example, dif-ferentiating between what a character t hinks and knows is essential for the interpretation of a text. Cognitive words can provide a medium through which a character’s mental states can be interpreted, as well as through which the past, present, and future goals and motives of that character can be analyzed. Cognitive words allow the reader to designate and reflect on what is true or false, real or unreal, and ambiguous or unambiguous in text. Thus, cognitive words have special salience in under-standing and evaluating written language.In summary, children have difficulty in mastering the cognitive word lexicon for at least three reasons. First, since the cognitive word lexicon is very intricate, subtle contrasts and comparisons must be learned in order forcognitive words to be used appropriately (HallHall, 1991). Second, since cognitive words are abstract and elusive and deal with the inner workings of the mind, they may not benefit easily from nonlinguistic strategies (Clark, 1983). Third, even though exposure to cognitive words appears to be essential for their efficient acquisition (Beeghly et al., 1986; Olson many children are exposed to cognitive words in written or oral form infrequently (Astingtoncorrelate more highly with Verbal than with Quantitative achievementscores; (c) correlate more highly withReading Comprehension and Cognitive Words 7bulary than with Reading Comprehension achievement scores; and (d) that there would be a lower correlation between high-frequency cognitive words with achievement scores than would be found for low-frequency cognitive words.Second, we wanted to investigate the hier-archical taxonomy of cognitive words proposed by Frank and Hall (199 1) in order to study its relationship to reading comprehension. In this connection we made several predictions: (a)cognitive word knowledge would increase with age; (b) the acquisition of cognates of think would be earlier than the acquisition of cog-nates of know; and (c) high levels of meaning would be acquired after low levels of meaning.SubjectsMETHODSubjects represented elementary, middle-, and high-school, and college levels. The grade-school subjects attended different single-gender private schools in the Washington, DC metro-politan area. There were 31 fifth-grade stu-dents,grade students,age = 15.6;SD = 0.5). The mean ages of the males and females within grades were not significantly different; their data are combined for presenta-tion. The 70 undergraduate students, Hall, 1991). Of those two stories, there was one story with a low-frequency and one with a high-frequencyreplacement cognitive word (see Appendix A).Each replacement cognitive word was con-tained within one of four multiple-choice sentences following the story. The subject was asked to read the story and to then choose theNATIONAL READING RESEARCH CENTER, READING RESEARCH REPORT NO. 148James R. Booth & William S. HallTable 1. High- and Low-Frequency Replacement Cognitive Words for Each Level of Meaning for Thinkandsemous words in this study for several reasons. First, several studies have focused on the acquisition of know and think (Macnamara et al., 1976; JohnsonHall, 1991). Fourth, both know and think lend themselves to multiple meanings that can be inferred from their context of use.The replacement cognitive words for know and think that were included in the list of choices were selected from previous studies ofcognitive words (HalltonReading Comprehension and Cognitive Words9 Roget’s Thesaurus.Cognitive words weredivided into the six levels of meaning accord-ing to three independent raters’ judgments oftheir typical use. The three raters were gradu-ate students familiar with the Frank and Hall(1991) level of meaning hierarchy. If the threeraters did not agree on the level assigned to aparticular cognitive word, that word was dis-carded from the list. Frequencies of theseprospective cognitive words were collectedfrom the Hall et al. (1984) spoken languagecorpus, the Thorndike and Lorge (1944) writ-ten word corpus, and the Carroll et al. (1971)school textbook corpus.The low- and high-frequency replacementcognitive words were randomly chosen from acognitive word list created in the followingmanner: The most and least frequent words ineach level of meaning were considered outliersand were eliminated. Then, each level wasdivided in half by the median frequency cogni-tive word (based on Carroll et al., 1971) andthis median frequency was eliminated.frequency cognitive word passages; therefore,the subject was not unnecessarily confused bythe multitude of distractor items. After all, thepurpose of the cognitive word task was to10James R. Booth & William S. HallNote.“Unstandardized alpha was used because we added raw scores to form a composite score beforestandardization; calculation because mean differences between ages can inflate this coefficient; ‘alpha reliability was calculated with the level of meaningthese were printed on the first page of the task booklet (see Appendix A)scores. (A reliability of a=Furthermore, five out ofthe six reliability coefficients for the main ef-fects were greater than a =Reading Comprehension and Cognitive Words11ability lower limit was set at a=andplanning (a= cant correlations of the cognitive word total with achievement scores suggesting that the cognitive word task was validly measuring both the construct of verbal ability and the academic achievement. Correlations between cognitive word total and achievement scores were tested for significant differences using Steiger’s t-test for dependent r’s (1980). For alland higher (t= 1.56, for Vo-cabulary (r =p than for Quantitative(r =pConvergent/ discriminant construct validity was revealed by the fact that cognitive word total correlated higher with Verbal than with Quantitative achievement scores for the undergraduates and higher with Vocabulary than with Reading Comprehension achievement scores for the grade-school subjects. Furthermore, the corre-lations with cognitive word total were higher for low than for high-frequency cognitive words (see Table 3 for these correlations). Note that the correlations were typically lower for the seventh-graders. This relative lack of association may be attributed to the12James R. Booth. Grades Means and All Groups Means adjusted for mean differences between ageclasses. Since cognitive word total was significantly different between grades, pooling the different age classes may lower its correlation coefficient with the achievement scores variables: therefore, the cognitive word mean of each age class was subtracted from the individual scores within it.total would correlate more highly with Verbal Taxonomy of Cognitive Words Proposed by than Quantitative achievement scores, and Frank and Hall (1991)more highly with Vocabulary than ReadingComprehension achievement scores were Since the academic ability of each age level supported. Moreover, our prediction thathigher than high-frequency cognitive wordswith Verbal, Quantitative,Reading Comprehension and Cognitive Words13Note.deviation. Grade-schoolers’ scores are ERB independent school norm percentiles;undergraduate scores are SAT-scaled scores. GPA is on a 4-point scale.lary , and Reading Comprehension achievement scores revealed no age differences in achieve-ment scores. The grade-schoolers’withthe Readability Index, the Fog Index, the average number of syllables per word, or the average number of words per sentence as dependent variables revealed no significantdifferences. (The Right Writer computer pro-gram was used to compute these dependent variables.) In addition, the reading difficulty of cognitive word passages could not account for the mean percent-correct difference observed between cognitive word passages. Correlations between the mean percent-correct for the cognitive word passages and the above reading difficulty indices for those passages were insignificant within each age level and across all age levels. Therefore, all age, frequency,think or know, and level of meaning differences must be accounted for by cognitive word knowledge, and not the reading difficulty of the passages. (However, the reading compre-hension abilities necessary for reading these passages may account for some of the variance explained in achievement scores by the cogni-tive word task).14James R. Booththe youngest age group in our study. These low readability indices sug-gest that reading comprehension abilities, as opposed to cognitive word knowledge, were a minor factor in the students’ performance on these passages. Furthermore, the readability indices for all of the cognitive passages were below the eighth-grade level, suggesting that reading comprehension abilities were even lessof a factor in the performance of thewas computed. For this cognition levels were combined to form the high level of meaning category in order to attain at least four cognitive word passages per comparison group necessary for sufficient reliability. The means and standard deviations by grade for cognitive word task subscales are presented in Table 5. We subtracted 0.33 for each wrong answer to correct for guessing; negative scores were counted as zero (see Astingtoncorrect to enable the comparison of subscales that had different numbers of passages within them.All main effects were significant.was significantly greaterthan percent-correct for high level ofmeaningPercent-correct for== 20.94, p <.OOl) than low-frequency cognitive words Percent-correct forthink (M =1,156) = 26.11, thanknowPost hocwith Scheffe range comparisonfifth-, seventh-, and tenth-graders scored significantly lower than undergraduates on low level of meaning and think; (c) fifth-graders scored significantly lower than tenth-graders on low level of meaning, think, and high-frequen-cy; and (d) fifth- and seventh-graders scored significantly lower than undergraduates on high-frequency. Our expectations that develop-mental differences would exist in cognitive word acquisition, that low-frequency would be acquired after high-frequency, that high level of meaning would be acquired after low level of meaning, and that cognates of think would be acquired after cognates of know were sup-ported.Reading Comprehension and Cognitive Words15think, low level of meaning was significantly greater inp < ==><Reading Comprehension and Cognitive Words 17Figure 2. Three-way interaction of Think or Know, Frequency, and Grade. Note: High or Low Freq.(frequency in written language),= 10.03,see FigureIn sum, there was a devel-opmental trend of increasing cognitive word knowledge with a period of accelerated cogni-tive word acquisition after seventh-grade andbefore the undergraduate level. Within grade analyses indicated: (a) for fifth-graders, low level of meaning think was answered correctly significantly more often than the other three;(b) for seventh-graders, low level of meaning think was answered correctly significantly more than high level of meaning think; (c) for tenth-graders, no significant differences were found; and (d) for undergraduates, low level of meaning know was answered incorrectly sig-nificantly more than the other three In sum, low level of meaning think appeared to be easier for the fifth- and seventh-graders, and18James R. Booth & William S. HallFigure 3. Three-way interaction of Frequency, Level of Meaning, and Grade. Note: High or Low Frequency (frequency in written language), Low or High Level (level of meaning).high level of meaning know appeared to be harder for undergraduates, while all combina-tions appeared to be equal in difficulty for tenth-graders (see Figure 1).The Think/Knowx Frequency interactionrevealed that athigh frequencies,think was signifi-cantly greater in percent-correct than know (M=and know (M=In other words, different frequencies did not significantly affect percent-correct forknowbut for think,low-frequencylowered percent-correct as compared tohigh-frequency For the Think/Know x Frequency x Gradeinteraction seeFiguregraders scored significantly lower than under-graduates on high-frequencythinkReading Comprehension and Cognitive Words 19tion after seventh and before tenth grade.Within grade analyses indicated: (a) forfor tenth-graders, no significantdifferences were found; and (c) for undergrad-uates, high- and low-frequency think were answered correctly significantly more thanhigh- and low-frequency know= 37.29, revealed that at high frequencies, high (M=levels of meaning did notsignificantly differ in percent-correctwas significantly lower inbut for high level of meaning,low-frequency cognitive words significantlylowered percent-correct as compared to= 3.60, psee Figure<the data were collapsed across frequency and according to whether the word was think or know. This analysis revealed significant maineffects for grade,156) = 12.20,p . There was no significant interaction.Post hoc one-waywere computed.Between level of meaning analyses indicated that perception (M=and memory (M=tion (M = Between grade analyses indicated that fifth- (M = scored significantly lower in percent-correct than tenth-graders (M=Then, post hoc one-wayNATIONAL READING RESEARCH CENTER, READING RESEARCH REPORT NO.1420James R. Booth & William S. Halltion fifth- and seventh-graders scored signifi-cantly lower than undergraduates, and formemory and evaluation, fifth-graders scoredsignificantly lower than tenth-graders. Con-trary to expectations, seventh-graders scoredsignificantly lower for metacognition thanOlson, 1990); therefore,we know little about cognitive word knowledgein older children and adults. Second, despitethe voluminous research on internal statewords, only two studies have assessed knowl-edge of more than three cognitive words at onetime (AstingtonTorrance, 1986). It is essential to assess a widevariety of cognitive words to uncover theimpact of the mental state lexicon on cognitivedevelopment. Third, only three studies haverelated cognitive words to reading comprehen-sion empirically (OlsonOlson, 1990). Fourth, weused a questionnaire to assess cognitive words,so we were not dependent on frequency ofReading Comprehension and Cognitive Words21Hall, 1991; Hall et al., 1987) is unique, as most experimental investigations of word meaning have employed concrete nouns to test psychological theories of word meaning (for an exception see Millerham, 1985). “The meaning of different words may be of different types: a single analysis of concepts may well not suffice” (Carey, 1982,p. 360). Therefore, the semantically complicat-ed lexicon of cognitive words may itself re-quire an equally intricate theory of acquisition (Scholnick, 1987).Cognitive Word Acquisition: Conceptualtion, metacognition, and planning were not.Frank and Hall (1991) then tried to incorporate the cognitive word think into their cognitive word hierarchy. They found that evaluation was used most often and that the other five levels of meaning were statistically undifferen-tiated for both children and adults.The present study provided some support for the Frank and Hall (1991) level of meaning hierarchy, as all of the level of meaning differ-ences were in the hypothesized direction. More specifically,perception was significantly great-er in percent-correct than metacognition, and memory was significantly greater inand between children and adults (Frank22James R. Boothgraders scored significantly lower than under-graduates. Contrary to expectations, forknow means “to perceive with certainty; to understand clearly; to be sure of or well-in-formed about; as we know the facts . . . to have a firm mental grasp of . . . to have clear and certain perception,to perform any mentaloperation; to reason.” From reflection on the preceding definitions, it is clear that to know means to have a definite, veridical understand-ing of something, whereas to think merely means to reflect about something, probably with an indefinite, uncertain understanding. In short, knowing what another person thinks is much easier than knowing what another person knows. Our discrepancy with Frank and Hall (1991) may also be accounted for, in part, by the fact that our study measured comprehension while theirs measured word production. How-ever, Hughes (1985) found the correlation between production and comprehension of cognitive words to be rather high (r = X Levelof Meaning x Grade (see Figure 1) interac-tions also illuminate the Frank and Hall (1991)conceptual difficulty and prototypicality hy-pothesis. Closer analysis of Figure 1 revealsNATIONAL READING RESEARCH CENTER, READING RESEARCH REPORT NO. 14Reading Comprehension and Cognitive Words23that the two-way interaction appears to hold only for fifth- and seventh-graders; that is, low level of meaning think is easier than high level of meaning think, and high and low level of meaning know. In contrast, there appear to be no significant differences in tenth-graders, and only the two main effects exist in undergradu-ates. Again, Frank and Hall (199 1) suggest that think has a prototypical meaning at a higher level of meaning than know and that is why it is learned later. Our only significant differ-ences were that children learned the low level of meaning think earlier than high level of meaning think, low level of meaning know, and high level of meaning know. According to the Frank and Hall (1991) hypothesis, this indi-cates, at least for the fifth- and seventh-grad-ers, that the prototypical level of meaning of think may be at the lower and not the higher level of meaning, and that know may not have a prototypical level of meaning. Further re-search must address this question.The two-way Think/Know X Frequency andthree-wayports a high correlation between adult and child use of cognitive words (Hall et al., 1987;Beeghly et al., 1986).The Frequency X Level of Meaning inter-action and the Frequency x Level of Meaning x Grade interaction (see Figure 3) revealed that all grades except tenth appeared to havedifficulty with high level of meaningx Level of Mean-ing x Frequency interaction. The 4 (Grade) x 2 (Frequency) x 2 (Think/Know) x 2 (Levelof Meaning)it would be meaningless; two cognitive word passages per cell does not allow for sufficient reliability. Nevertheless, from the previously described three two-way interactions it appears that the high-frequency and low level of meaning think are relatively easy at least for fifth- and seventh-graders, while low-frequencyNATIONAL READING RESEARCH CENTER,。