社区心理学的研究方法

合集下载

心理学研究报告方法总结

心理学研究报告方法总结

心理学研究报告方法总结1.实验方法:实验方法是心理学研究中最常用的方法之一、它通过控制和操纵变量来研究因果关系。

实验通常包括实验组和控制组,研究者对实验组施以特定的干预措施,然后通过比较实验组和控制组的差异来得出结论。

2.调查方法:调查方法是一种用于收集心理学数据的常用方法。

调查方法可以采用问卷调查、面谈、观察等方式进行,通过收集被试者的主观反应和行为数据来了解心理现象。

调查方法一般包括问卷设计、采样、数据收集和数据分析等步骤。

3.观察方法:观察方法是通过观察和记录行为来研究心理现象的方法。

观察方法可以采用自然观察或实验室观察的方式进行。

观察方法通常需要严谨的观察者培训和工作协议,以确保数据的可靠性和有效性。

4.病例研究方法:病例研究方法是通过深入研究个别个体或群体来了解心理现象的方法。

病例研究方法通常包括对个体或群体的细致观察、面谈和心理测量等方法。

病例研究方法适用于研究特殊心理现象或个体,但结果不能推广到整体人群。

5.实地研究方法:实地研究方法是指研究者亲自到研究对象所在的现场进行研究的方法。

这种方法可以提供真实和详尽的数据,但也面临着研究者的主观制约和实践困难等问题。

6.实时测量方法:实时测量方法是指在研究过程中使用技术设备对心理现象进行实时测量的方法。

这种方法可以获得准确和详尽的数据,但也需要研究者具备相应的技术能力和设备支持。

总之,心理学研究报告方法的选择应根据研究目的和问题进行合理的考虑。

不同的方法可以提供不同类型的数据和信息,研究者需要结合具体情况来选择最适合的方法。

同时,在进行研究报告时,还需要严格按照科学的方法和规范进行数据收集、数据处理和结果呈现,以确保研究结果的可靠性和有效性。

社区心理学的新视角分析(二)2024

社区心理学的新视角分析(二)2024

社区心理学的新视角分析(二)引言概述:社区心理学是一门探索社区中个体心理特征和社会环境的相互关系的学科。

本文将以新视角分析社区心理学,从以下五个大点展开探讨:(1)社区的共同认同和社会支持;(2)社区发展和个体幸福感;(3)社区心理健康的促进;(4)社区的社会支持网络;(5)社区参与和社会资源配置。

正文:1. 社区的共同认同和社会支持1.1 社区认同的定义和意义1.2 社区认同与身份认同的关系1.3 社会支持在社区认同中的作用1.4 社区认同的影响因素1.5 提升社区认同的方法2. 社区发展和个体幸福感2.1 社区发展对个体幸福感的影响2.2 社区环境对个体幸福感的影响2.3 个体幸福感的测量指标2.4 社区发展与个体幸福感的关系2.5 促进社区发展和个体幸福感的策略3. 社区心理健康的促进3.1 社区心理健康的概念和重要性3.2 社区环境对心理健康的影响3.3 社区心理健康的评估方法3.4 社区心理健康的促进策略3.5 社区心理健康服务的发展方向4. 社区的社会支持网络4.1 社会支持网络的定义和构成4.2 社会支持网络的类型4.3 社会支持网络的功能和作用4.4 社会支持网络的形成和维系4.5 优化社会支持网络的措施和建议5. 社区参与和社会资源配置5.1 社区参与的概念和意义5.2 社区参与对社会资源配置的影响5.3 社区参与的层次和形式5.4 社区参与的挑战和障碍5.5 提升社区参与度的策略和方法总结:社区心理学的新视角为我们提供了更深入的理解社区与个体心理的关系。

通过探讨社区的共同认同和社会支持、社区发展和个体幸福感、社区心理健康的促进、社区的社会支持网络以及社区参与和社会资源配置等方面,我们能够更好地认识社区心理学的实践意义,为社区的发展和个体的幸福作出更有效的贡献。

社 会心理学实验设计

社 会心理学实验设计

社会心理学实验设计一、实验背景社会心理学是研究个体和群体在社会相互作用中的心理和行为发生及变化规律的学科。

通过实验方法,可以更深入地探究社会心理现象的成因和影响机制。

本次实验旨在研究社会情境对个体助人行为的影响。

二、实验目的本实验的目的是考察在不同的社会情境下,个体的助人行为是否会有所不同。

具体而言,我们想要探究以下两个问题:1、旁观者数量对个体助人行为的影响。

2、紧急程度对个体助人行为的影响。

三、实验假设基于以往的研究和理论,我们提出以下假设:假设 1:旁观者数量越多,个体提供帮助的可能性越低。

假设 2:紧急程度越高,个体提供帮助的可能性越高。

四、实验方法(一)被试通过在大学校园和社区发布招募广告,招募了 200 名年龄在 18-60岁之间的志愿者作为被试。

被试被随机分配到不同的实验条件中。

(二)实验设计本实验采用 2×2 被试间设计,自变量为旁观者数量(多、少)和紧急程度(高、低),因变量为被试是否提供帮助以及提供帮助的及时性。

(三)实验材料1、实验场景的设置:在一个模拟的公共场所(如公园、商场等),布置不同的场景以体现旁观者数量和紧急程度的差异。

2、求助信号:设计明显的求助信号,如呼喊、手势等。

(四)实验程序1、被试到达实验地点后,先进行简单的培训,告知他们实验的大致流程和注意事项,但不透露实验的真正目的。

2、被试被随机引导到不同的实验场景中。

3、在每个场景中,当预设的求助信号发出后,观察被试的反应,并记录被试是否提供帮助以及提供帮助的时间。

五、实验结果预期如果假设 1 成立,我们预期在旁观者数量多的条件下,被试提供帮助的比例会低于旁观者数量少的条件。

如果假设 2 成立,在紧急程度高的条件下,被试提供帮助的比例和及时性都会高于紧急程度低的条件。

六、实验可能遇到的问题及解决方法(一)被试猜测到实验目的为了避免被试猜测到实验目的,我们在实验前的培训中尽量模糊实验的真正意图,同时在实验场景的设置和求助信号的设计上尽量做到自然和真实。

心理学研究方法

心理学研究方法

心理学研究方法心理学研究方法是心理学领域中至关重要的一部分,它为心理学家们提供了系统、科学的研究框架,使他们能够深入了解人类心理活动的规律和特点。

在心理学研究中,科学的研究方法是确保研究结果可靠性和有效性的关键。

本文将介绍心理学研究方法的基本概念、常用的研究方法以及其在心理学研究中的应用。

首先,心理学研究方法的基本概念包括实证研究和理论研究。

实证研究是指通过实验、观察、调查等手段,收集数据并进行分析,从而验证或推翻假设的研究方式。

而理论研究则是通过对心理学理论的建构、发展和修正,来揭示心理现象的内在规律和本质特征。

这两种研究方法相辅相成,共同推动了心理学领域的发展。

在心理学研究中,常用的研究方法包括实验研究、观察研究和调查研究。

实验研究是通过对被试验者进行控制实验,观察和测量其行为和心理活动,从而得出结论的研究方法。

观察研究是通过对被试验者进行自然观察或实验室观察,记录和分析其行为和心理活动的研究方法。

调查研究是通过问卷调查、访谈等方式,收集和分析大量数据,了解人们的态度、观念、行为等的研究方法。

这些研究方法在心理学研究中各有所长,可以根据研究目的和对象的不同,选择合适的研究方法进行研究。

在心理学研究中,这些研究方法被广泛应用于各个领域。

比如在临床心理学领域,实验研究被用于研究心理疾病的发病机制和治疗方法;在教育心理学领域,观察研究被用于研究学习动机和学习策略;在社会心理学领域,调查研究被用于研究人们的社会态度和行为。

这些研究方法的应用,丰富了心理学的研究内容,拓展了心理学的研究领域,为人们提供了更多关于心理活动的科学知识。

总之,心理学研究方法是心理学研究的基础和核心,它为心理学家们提供了科学的研究框架和方法,使他们能够深入了解人类心理活动的规律和特点。

通过实证研究和理论研究,心理学家们可以揭示心理现象的内在规律和本质特征,为人们提供更多关于心理活动的科学知识。

因此,心理学研究方法的不断完善和发展,将为心理学领域的进一步发展和人类心理活动的深入了解提供重要支持。

社会心理学研究-从众心理

社会心理学研究-从众心理

抑制创新思维
创新思维
从众心理可能导致个体在思考和 决策时过于依赖群体观点,从而 抑制了个体的创新思维和独立思
考能力。
缺乏判性思考
当个体倾向于跟随群体意见时,他 们可能缺乏批判性思考,不会对信 息进行深入分析和评估。
抑制独特观点
从众心理可能导致群体中缺乏独特 和新颖的观点,因为个体往往不愿 意提出与大多数人不同的意见。
3
群体影响
广告商可以利用群体影响,通过口碑营 销、社交媒体推广等方式,让消费者觉 得大多数人都喜欢某个产品或服务,从 而影响他们的购买决策。
公共舆论引导
媒体报道
通过媒体报道,政府或组织可以 引导公众舆论,使人们相信大多 数人支持某观点或政策。例如, 强调民意调查结果或群体意见。
社会运动
利用从众心理,发起社会运动或 倡导特定价值观。例如,通过大 规模集会或游行,让人们觉得大
解释
从众心理是一种普遍存在的社会 心理现象,它涉及到个体在社会 生活中如何处理与他人的关系, 以及如何适应社会环境。
特性
群体压力
个体感受到来自群体的压力,这种压力可能 来自于他人的评价、看法或行为。
行为一致性
个体倾向于与群体保持一致,避免被视为与 众不同或不合群。
自我保护
个体为了避免被孤立或受到排斥,倾向于选 择符合群体期望的行为。
04
从众心理的现实应用
广告营销
1
广告策略
利用从众心理,广告商可以通过强调大 多数人都选择某产品或服务来吸引消费 者。例如,使用“销量领先”、“超过 90%的人选择我们”等广告语。
2
品牌形象
通过塑造品牌形象,广告商可以影响消 费者的选择。例如,通过广告展示某品 牌被广泛接受和喜爱,从而促使消费者 选择该品牌。

社区服务的心理学技巧分析

社区服务的心理学技巧分析

社区服务的心理学技巧分析
社区服务的心理学技巧包括以下方面:
1. 积极倾听:了解居民的真实需求和感受。

倾听可以让居民觉得被尊重和被关心,从而更容易与社区服务人员建立信任和亲近感。

2. 非语言沟通:使用适当的肢体语言和面部表情来传达友好和开放的态度。

例如,微笑、眼神接触和身体姿势等。

3. 沟通技巧:使用有效的沟通技巧,例如:提出开放式的问题、使用体贴的语言、清晰地表述和确认理解等。

4. 情感支持:提供情感支持、共情和鼓励。

在社区服务中,人们可能会面临种种压力和挑战,社区服务人员可以提供安慰、理解和支持,以帮助他们克服困难。

5. 解决问题的能力:提供有针对性和实际的解决问题方案,并与居民合作进行实施和评估。

这有助于增强社区居民的自我效能感和独立性。

6. 资源整合:了解社区内的资源和服务,并适当地引导和推荐居民。

综合利用社区内的资源可以帮助居民更好地解决问题和满足需求。

总之,社区服务的心理学技巧可以增强社区服务人员的专业能力和服务质量,从
而更好地满足社区居民的需求。

社会心理学研究方法

社会心理学研究方法
她往往就是进一步研究得前提。要对一种复 杂社会心理现象进行研究,首先必须了解这 一现象得本质特征及其与其她现象得区别。
现象揭示研究就是典型得回答“就是什么” 和“怎么样”等问题得研究。
关系解释研究
关系解释研究又称相关研究,就是考察两个 或更多变量(即现象)得相互关系,揭示一个变 量就是否受其她变量影响,影响程度和性质 如何,进而用一个变量预测另一个变量得研 究方法。
现场实验 实验室实验 系统观察 调查(包括问卷和访问) 测验 跨文化研究 档案研究 模拟研究 个案研究 统计分析
阅读:《街角社会:一个意大利人贫民区得 社会结构》
社会学家威廉·富特·怀特在1936-1940•年间对美国波士顿得 一个意大利人贫民区得研究以及在此基础之上写成得《街角社 会:一个意大利人贫民区得社会结构》,就是运用参与观察法得 典范之作。在从事研究得那些日子里,怀特以被研究群体 ──“街角帮”一员得身份,置身于被研究者得生活环境和日常 活动之中,对闲荡于街头巷尾得意裔青年得生活状况、非正式组 织得内部结构、活动方式,以及她们与周围社会得关系加以观察, 并及时作出记录与分析,最后得出了关于该社区社会结构及互动 方式得重要结论。怀特写道:“街角帮得结构产生于帮得成员之 间长时期得经常交往。多数帮得核心得形成可以追溯到成员们 得少年时代,……街角青年也可能从这个地区内得某一处搬到另 一处居住,但就是她几乎总就是会继续忠于她最初得街角。”
两个变量之间出现相关,可以有三种情况:第一个变 量得变化引起了第二个变量得变化;第二个变量得 变化引起了第一个变量得变化;一个没有指明得第 三变量同时引起了两个变量得变化。
因果联系证实研究
因果联系证实研究得目得就是确定不同变量 之间得因果联系,通常就是为验证设想得因 果联系或假说。

心理学研究的方法

心理学研究的方法

心理学研究的方法观察法是心理学中最基本的研究方法之一、观察法通过直接观察人们的行为、言语和表情等,来了解其心理过程和行为模式。

观察法可以是自然观察,即观察人们在自然环境中真实的行为;也可以是人工观察,研究者创造一种特定的环境,观察参与者在该环境下的行为。

观察法的优点是反映现实环境中的行为,结果具有较高的外部有效性;缺点是无法获得被观察者的内部感受和思维过程,结果的内部有效性较低。

实验法是心理学中最常用的研究方法之一、实验法通过对人们的操作性变量进行系统地操纵,来观察其对心理过程和行为的影响。

实验法可以分为实验组和对照组,通过对比两组的结果,确定操作性变量对心理过程和行为的影响。

实验法的优点是能够控制其他变量,更精确地推断因果关系;缺点是实验环境无法完全模拟现实环境,结果的外部有效性较低。

问卷调查法是通过向被试者发放调查问卷,让其填写相关问题并提供反馈,来获取大量信息的方法。

问卷调查法可以通过封闭式问题获取定量数据,也可以通过开放式问题获取定性数据。

问卷调查法的优点是可以涵盖大样本量和广泛的研究对象;缺点是受到被试者自觉性和主观性的影响,结果的真实性和准确性有一定风险。

临床访谈法是一种通过面对面的沟通,与被试者进行深入的探讨和交流,以了解其个人经历、情感和意愿的方法。

临床访谈法常用于临床心理学领域,用于诊断和治疗心理问题。

临床访谈法的优点是能够获取被试者的主观经验和内心感受;缺点是受到访谈师的主观解释和判断的影响,结果的客观性和可靠性有一定挑战。

个案研究法是通过深入地研究一个案例,来揭示特定个体的心理特征和过程的方法。

个案研究法通常结合多种数据收集方法,如观察、访谈、问卷等,来获取全面和深入的信息。

个案研究法的优点是可以研究特定个体的个别特征和经历;缺点是结果的普遍性和可推广性较低。

综上所述,心理学研究的方法具有各自的特点和适用范围。

研究者可以根据具体的研究目的和条件,选择适合自己的研究方法,从而有效地开展心理学的研究工作。

  1. 1、下载文档前请自行甄别文档内容的完整性,平台不提供额外的编辑、内容补充、找答案等附加服务。
  2. 2、"仅部分预览"的文档,不可在线预览部分如存在完整性等问题,可反馈申请退款(可完整预览的文档不适用该条件!)。
  3. 3、如文档侵犯您的权益,请联系客服反馈,我们会尽快为您处理(人工客服工作时间:9:00-18:30)。

American Journal of Community Psychology,Vol.35,Nos.3/4,June2005(C 2005)DOI:10.1007/s10464-005-3397-zGetting the Big Picture in Community Science:Methods That Capture ContextDouglas A.Luke1Community science has a rich tradition of using theories and research designs that are con-sistent with its core value of contextualism.However,a survey of empirical articles publishedin the American Journal of Community Psychology shows that community scientists utilize anarrow range of statistical tools that are not well suited to assess contextual data.Multilevelmodeling,geographic information systems(GIS),social network analysis,and cluster analysisare recommended as useful tools to address contextual questions in community science.Anargument for increased methodological consilience is presented,where community scientistsare encouraged to adopt statistical methodology that is capable of modeling a greater pro-portion of the data than is typical with traditional methods.KEY WORDS:community science;context;multilevel modeling;GIS;social network analysis;clusteranalysis.INTRODUCTIONA generation ago,Allen Barton had this to say about social science research:For the last thirty years,empirical social research hasbeen dominated by the sample survey.But as usuallypracticed,using random sampling of individuals,thesurvey is a sociological meatgrinder,tearing the indi-vidual from his social context and guaranteeing thatnobody in the study interacts with anyone else in it.It is a little like a biologist putting his experimen-tal animals through a hamburger machine and look-ing at every hundredth cell through a microscope;anatomy and physiology get lost,structure and func-tion disappear,and one is left with cell biology....If our aim is to understand people’s behavior ratherthan simply to record it,we want to know about pri-mary groups,neighborhoods,organizations,socialcircles,and communities;about interaction,com-munication,role expectations,and social control.(Barton,1968as reported in Freeman,2004)Although this statement came a few years after the Swampscott Conference,it is almost as if Barton were talking to the group of community scientists1To whom correspondence should be addressed at Saint Louis University School of Public Health,3545Lafayette Avenue,Saint Louis,MO63104;e-mail:dluke@.who were in Massachusetts inventing a newfield.As Kelly notes,“The conference was an occasion to ac-claim that beyond conventional methods and,with a focus beyond the individual,there were valid activi-ties and meaningful roles for a new kind of psychol-ogist,the community psychologist(Kelly,2002,p.44, emphasis added).Thus,community scientists have put context front and center as one of the core values of community psychology.Shinn and Rapkin(2000) advised that“...a central tenet of community psy-chology is that human behavior must be under-stood in context.”So,for example,community sci-entists study domestic violence using methods and theories that are consistent with the view that do-mestic violence is not just an individual behavior, but a complex process shaped by historical,social,financial,and legal contexts.The types of groups that community scientists work with(e.g.,domes-tic violence victims,young mothers,gays and les-bians,drug users,inner city residents,etc.)are of interest not because of any defining psychological characteristics,but because these groups have been affected in specific ways by the economic,social, cultural,and physical situations in which they are embedded.1850091-0562/05/0600-0185/0C 2005Springer Science+Business Media,Inc.186LukeHowever,although community scientists fre-quently employ theories,models,and frameworks that take context into account,they seem to be less likely to employ contextual methods in their work. Community psychology does have a long tradition of analytic innovation(Revenson et al.,2002),but the majority of empirical work in community science utilizes a remarkably narrow array of analytic ap-proaches.In an earlier set of articles examining science and community psychology,Kelly’s(2003)first sug-gestion for vitalizing scientific community psychology was to“demythologize statistics.”He argued that not only do we need to move beyond traditional methods such as ANOVA,regression,and factor analysis;but that community scientists need to be in control of the quantitative methods,and not vice versa.This paper is one small response to his call.The goal of this paper is to provide examples of a number of useful analytic methods that can be used to capture community context.It is my hope that if community scientists have a wider variety of analytic tools in their toolbox,they will be more likely to get away from Barton’s sample survey approach to do-ing social science.To support this argument,the next section presents a review of statistical practices dur-ing six years of articles published in the American Journal of Community Psychology.The bulk of the paper describes four statistical methods appropriate for assessing context,and provides examples drawn from community science.The paper concludes with a discussion of the need for the development of con-silient statistical methods in community science. STATISTICAL PRACTICE INCOMMUNITY SCIENCETo provide context for the following discussion, I conducted a review of the types of statistical meth-ods used in the American Journal of Community Psy-chology(AJCP)during two time periods:from1981 to1983,and20years later from2001to2003.By re-viewing six years of AJCP articles,we can get a good sense of the types of statistical practices used by com-munity scientists.Also,by examining two different time periods we can see how statistical practice has changed over a20year period.For the purposes of this paper,this review can help ascertain the extent to which contextual methods have been used in the past, or are being used currently by AJCP authors.Al-though AJCP is not the only place that community-oriented empirical work appears,it is one of the more important settings for work that aims to advance community science.A total of304articles were reviewed;144in the early period(1981–1983)and160in the current period(2001–2003).Table I shows how the types of articles published in AJCP have changed during this time.In the early period,the majority(88%) of published articles were traditional empirical stud-ies where original data were presented.The current AJCP publishes a wider variety of articles.Almost half of all articles(48%)came from themed special issues.More room was also made for Presidential and award addresses.Finally,on a percentage basis four times as many articles with qualitative content are be-ing currently published compared to20years earlier (17%vs.4%).Figure1shows how often particular types of statistical analyses were used in the215empirical papers published in the early1980s and from2001 to2003.(These percentages add up to more than 100%,because most papers used multiple types of statistical analyses.)An empirical article is one that presents original data that have been analyzed either quantitatively or qualitatively.We can see that there have been some changes in statistical practice over the past two decades.Well more than half(59.5%) of all empirical articles presented correlations in the early80s;currently a little over a third(34.8%)of the empirical articles present correlations.The use of ANOVA methods shows a similar drop,from 59.5to37.1%.Use of structural equation modeling (SEM)and associated methods such as path analysis increased dramatically during this same period(from 2.4to11.2%).This is not surprising given that sophis-ticated SEM software did not appear until the1990s.However,despite these changes,thisfigure supports Kelly’s contention that as afield weTable I.AJCP Article CharacteristicsYears Total articles Empirical Qualitative Special issue Address 1981–1983144126(88%)6(4%)10(7%)5(4%) 2001–200316089(56%)27(17%)76(48%)19(12%) Totals304215(71%)33(11%)86(28%)24(8%)Getting the Big Picture in Community Science:Methods that Capture Context187Fig.1.Statistics usage in the American Journal of Community Psychology from 1981to 1983(N =126empirical articles)andfrom 2001to 2003(N =89empirical articles).predominately use traditional statistical approaches such as ANOVA,regression,psychometrics,corre-lations,and categorical statistics (e.g.,chi-square).More specialized techniques such as cluster analysis,social network analysis (SNA),multilevel modeling (HLM),meta-analysis,non-linear modeling,and ge-ographic information systems (GIS)are used rarely.Of the four techniques that are the topic of this paper (i.e.,cluster analysis,social network analysis,multi-level modeling,and geographic information systems)only network analysis was used at all in the early 3-year period.Furthermore,none of these contextual techniques appeared in more than four studies from 2001to 2003.Although one would expect that a very general modeling technique such as ANOVA would be used more often than,say,network analysis,the generality of the technique is not the entire story.For example,structural equation modeling,multi-level modeling,and nonlinear modeling are also very general statistical approaches,but they have not been used widely in the community sciences.One possible interpretation of the results from this methodologic review is that community science simply does not need to use nontraditional statistical tools to “...make a positive difference in commu-nity living and activities ...”(Sarason,2003,p.209).However,the main point I hope to make in the fol-lowing sections of this paper is not that we should use different tools because they are newer,more in-novative,or more popular.Rather,I believe that the quantitative methods that we historically rely on are not always the most appropriate tools if community scientists are seriously interested in understanding how the physical,social,organizational,cultural,eco-nomic,and political context shape human behavior and health.Although this paper will talk much about statistical practice,it is not a statistics paper.At its heart,I believe this argument is more philosophical and political than technical.Put simply,the decisions we make about the tools we use in community sci-ence say something about the values we hold as com-munity scientists.188LukeCONTEXTUALISM ANDCOMMUNITY SCIENCEI should venture to assert that the most pervasivefallacy of philosophic thinking goes back to neglectof context.–John Dewey As part of its mission statement,the Society for Community Research and Action(SCRA)recognizes that“Human competencies and problems are best understood by viewing people within their social,cul-tural,economic,geographic,and historical contexts”(SCRA,2004).This core value of contextualism can be seen in many ways,including the close ties of com-munity psychology to ecological psychology,the use of explicitly contextual theories such as Behavior Set-ting Theory(Wicker,1992),and the recognition that effective interventions based on community science can and should be aimed at the extra-individual level (Tseng et al.,2002).That is,changing families,neigh-borhoods,schools,churches,and organizations can be a more effective way to enhance health than sim-ply intervening at the person level.The long history of contextualism in commu-nity science puts us at the leading edge of a wave of change in how human health and behavior should be studied.One very prominent example is the2001 report issued by the National Institutes of Health (NIH)entitled Toward higher levels of analysis: Progress and promise in research on social and cul-tural dimensions of health(Office of Behavioral and Social Sciences Research,2001).This report pre-sented a new agenda for NIH research focusing on two goals:(1)expanding health-related social sci-ences research at NIH and(2)integrating social sci-ence research into interdisciplinary contextual and multilevel studies of health.Particularly relevant for this discussion is the recommendation to:...support the development of state-of-the-art so-cial science methods.Challenges include measure-ment at the group,network,neighborhood,andcommunity levels;the further development of meth-ods for longitudinal research;multi-level researchdesigns that integrate diverse qualitative and quan-titative approaches...;and the development ofimproved methods for data collection and analysis(p.3).The empirical work published in AJCP shows the importance and prevalence of contextual ap-proaches.Notable examples in the past few years include interventions to enhance the quality of life of battered women through a community-based ad-vocacy approach(Bybee&Sullivan,2002);a de-scription of the relationship of ethnicity to family networks(Hirsch,Mickus,&Boerger,2002);iden-tification of supportive organizational characteris-tics for rape victim advocates(Wasco,Campbell, &Clark,2002),and a study examining the effects of organizational downsizing on the health of em-ployees(Kivim¨aki,Vahtera,Elovainio,Pentti,& Virtanen,2003).These(and many other)examples utilize explicitly contextual frameworks.However, closer examination of these studies reveals that al-though they use a contextual framework,the ac-tual data collection and analyses are restricted to the person-level.This is a general pattern.One hundred twenty of the empirical studies reviewed above incorporate some type of contextual framework or construct to understand individual-level behavior or character-istics.However,less than one in four(22.5%)of these studies collected data directly from the extra-individual levels or settings,or analyzed these data using appropriate methods for multiple levels.In other words,although community scientists value contextual thinking,we are much less likely to actu-ally employ contextual methods.There are a number of possible explanations for this disconnect between our theories and our meth-ods.First,community psychology is still a relatively youngfield,and many practitioners have received their training from people and places still rooted in the person-centered traditions of clinical and general psychology.Many of the research design and ana-lytic approaches which are appropriate for contex-tual studies are more commonly used in other dis-ciplines such as organizational behavior,sociology, urban planning,etc.Another reason for this disconnect that I would like to discuss at more length has to do with the Rule of the hammer—if the only tool you know how to use is a hammer,every situation looks like it needs to be hammered.The traditional statistical tools used most often by community scientists(i.e.,regression, ANOVA,factor analysis,etc.)have been developed to be used with a single level of analysis.Although it is common to use predictor variables in ANOVA and regression that give contextual information(e.g., group membership,state of residence,classroom), these models actually work by disaggregating group-level information to the individual level so that all predictors in a model are tied to the individual unit of analysis.This leads to at least two problems.First, all of the un-modeled contextual information endsGetting the Big Picture in Community Science:Methods that Capture Context189up pooled into the single individual error term of the model(Duncan,Jones,&Moon,1998).This is problematic because individuals belonging to the same context will presumably have correlated errors, which violates one of the basic assumptions of the general linear model.The second problem is that by ignoring context the model assumes that the re-gression coefficients apply equally to all contexts,“...thus propagating the notion that processes work out in the same way in different contexts”(Duncan et al.,1998,p.98).So,since we mainly know how to use statistics that are limited to individual-level analyses,it is not surprising that we end up not collecting or analyzing true contextual,multilevel data.STATISTICAL APPROACHES CONSISTENT WITH CONTEXTUALISMThe good news is that there are a number of quantitative tools available that are consistent with the value of contextualism,and that are extremely useful for describing or modeling the influence of ecological,environmental,or group-level factors on individual-level behavior or health.In the next sec-tions of this paper I would like to highlight four such methods:(1)multilevel modeling;(2)geographic in-formation systems(GIS);(3)social network analysis; and(4)cluster analysis.Multilevel ModelingMultilevel modeling is a general regression-based statistical tool that can build statistical mod-els of data that are multilevel in nature.Multilevel models are more statistically appropriate for multi-level data than are traditional regression or ANOVA techniques(Snijders&Bosker,1999).For example, if the data are truly multilevel in nature,then the level-1units are clustered within the level-2units, and the level-1error terms are not likely to be in-dependent,thus violating one of the basic assump-tions of the general linear model.More importantly for us,with multilevel modeling we can build statis-tical models that match our conceptual models.That is,there will no longer be the disconnect between our thinking and our methods.Multilevel modeling,also known as hierarchical linear modeling,mixed-effects modeling,or growth-curve modeling,has been underutilized in commu-nity science compared to developmental psychology, education,sociology,or political science.Hedeker, McMahon,Sason,and Salina(1994),in a study of a worksite smoking cessation program,gave an early demonstration of the utility of these methods;focus-ing on how the clustering of the person-level data can be appropriately adjusted for in a hierarchical model.Coulton,Korbin and Su(1999),used multi-level methods in an examination of how neighbor-hood factors influence child maltreatment.They con-vincingly demonstrated that the parameter estimates for the predictor variables were different than would be expected if only individual or neighborhood data had been used.Specifically,adverse neighborhood conditions weakened the effects of previously estab-lished individual risk factors,such as violence in the family of origin.Finally,Mankowski,Humphreys, and Moos(2001)develop a multilevel model that shows that person–environmentfit of treatment be-lief systems is an important predictor of the extent of 12-step group involvement.The following more detailed example may help demonstrate the utility of multilevel modeling for community science.The data and analyses come from work that we have done in the area of tobacco control policy(Luke&Krauss,2004).The main goal of this study was to identify the important influences on voting on tobacco-related legislation by members of Congress from1997to2000.The unit of analysis is an individual Congress member.A typical single-level regression model that could be developed for these data might look like this:VotePct i=β0+β1(Party)i+β2(Money)i+r i where we want to predict the percentage of time that a member of Congress voted in a pro-tobacco indus-try direction based on his or her political party,and the amount of money received from a tobacco indus-try political action committee(PAC).However,members of Congress are not ran-domly selected,and it is reasonable to expect that the data will exhibit cluster effects.That is,Sena-tors or Representatives within the same state such as Massachusetts are likely to be more similar to each other on any number of important characteristics than Congress members from two different states. Figure2illustrates this—we can see that the percent-age of time that a Congress member votes for a bill in a pro-tobacco industry direction varies from state to state.190LukeFig.2.Average pro-tobacco vote percentage by Congress members–1997–2000.The single-level regression model above cannothandle the clustering of the data(and the concomi-tant non-independence of error terms).More impor-tant than this statistical problem is the fact that as community scientists we would like to build a more contextual model.In particular,we would like to ac-count for state-level characteristics that may influ-ence voting behavior apart from the individual party and money received.The size of the tobacco econ-omy in a state,operationalized as size of tobacco har-vest,is a type of contextual variable that might influ-ence Congressional voting.The following set of equations show how a mul-tilevel statistical model is structured:VotePct=β0j+β1j(Party)ij+β2j(Money)ij+r ij β0j=γ00+γ01(Acres)j+u0jβ1j=γ10+γ11(Acres)j+u1jβ2j=γ20+γ21(Acres)j+u2jAlthough this statistical model looks quite a bit more complicated than the earlier single-level model, it is mainly doing two important things for us.First, the beta-coefficients at level-1become dependent variables at level-2—indicating that the level-1inter-cepts and slopes may vary from state to state.Sec-ond,this type of model clearly shows which predic-tor variables are acting at level-1(i.e.,political party, PAC money)and which at level-2(amount of to-bacco acreage produced in a state).Table II presents the results offitting this multi-level model.We can see that at the Congress member level being a Republican(ˆγ10=.5066)and receiv-ing more PAC money(ˆγ20=.0049)are associatedwith voting more often in the pro-tobacco indus-try direction.Specifically,for every$10,000received by a member of Congress from a tobacco PAC, there is approximately a5%greater chance to vote in the pro-tobacco industry direction on a tobacco-related bill.However,we also see that there is a sig-nificant state-level contextual effect—members from states with a greater tobacco economy are also more likely to vote pro-tobacco(ˆγ01=.0027).The rela-tively large and significant random variability com-ponent for the intercept term(u0j)suggests,more-over,that there may be other important contextual effects that have not been included in the model.This relatively simple multilevel model shows us that it is important to take both individual and contextual effects into account when modeling tobacco policy behavior in Congress.For more details on how toGetting the Big Picture in Community Science:Methods that Capture Context191 Table II.Hierarchical Model Estimates of the Effects of Political Party,Total Contributions,and State-Level Tobacco Acreage on Percentage of Pro-Tobacco VotesFixed effects Coefficient SE T-ratio pFor intercept(β0j)Intercept(γ00).1828.02058.90.000Acres(γ01).0027.0005 5.10.000For PARTY slope(β1j)Party(γ10).5066.021523.53.000Acres(γ11)−.0016.0004 3.60.001For MONEY slope(β2j)Money(γ20).0049.00058.80.000Acres(γ21)−.00002.0000 5.50.000Random effects SD p.χ2pIntercept(u0j).0978.009684.1.000Party Slope(u1j).0705.005054.8.023Money Slope(u2j).0009.000029.2>.50Level-1(e ij).1628.0265Model Fit Deviance Parameters AIC BIC−353.813−327.8−272.3Notes:Political party is coded0for Democrat and1for Republican.Contributions arerecorded in thousands of dollars and Acres are recorded in thousands of acres.fit and interpret multilevel models,see Hox(2002), Luke(2004),or Snijders and Bosker(1994). Geographic Information SystemsThe second useful quantitative tool for explor-ing context is geographic information systems(GIS). GIS is a set of database,mapping,and statistical tools that allow visual and quantitative assessment of geo-graphic information.(Geographic in the broad sense, meaning any type of information that has a physi-cal location.)Although the production of maps is a common end result of GIS methods,GIS can also be used to construct contextual variables and statis-tically analyze spatial relationships(Haining,2003). GIS methods emerged as a computing technology over the last several decades and are used extensively in a wide variety of areas including geology,meteo-rology,urban planning,public safety,marketing,po-litical science,and public health(Mark,Chrisman, Frank,McHaffie,&Pickles,2004).There has been a growing movement towards a‘community GIS’where GIS methods are used for community devel-opment,mapping of community assets,community health assessments,and eco-development(Carver, 2001).However,community scientists have been rel-atively slow to take advantage of GIS techniques.GIS has been used most often by health and social scientists to examine patterns of criminal and risky health behaviors.For example,Wieczorek and Hanson(1997)used GIS methods to examine pat-terns of driving-while-intoxicated(DWI)and reveal the geographic context of this behavior.The authors use GIS maps including contour plots which show that DWIs are not distributed randomly around the metropolitan area.This type of analysis leads to poli-cies and interventions that can be aimed more pre-cisely,leading to lower costs and hopefully enhanced effectiveness.A second example comes from the area of to-bacco control policy.Figure3is a map showing the distribution of tobacco billboards in the St Louis metropolitan area,shortly before tobacco billboards were eliminated as a provision of the1998Master Settlement Agreement(Luke,Esmundo,&Bloom, 2000).We used GIS to collect the billboard data and analyze the location patterns of tobacco advertising. In thefigure,the billboards are coded by the type of image found on the billboard,and the underly-ing map shows the population mix by census tract. By combining the billboard and census data,we can see that billboards with African American images on them tended to be concentrated in neighborhoods with higher proportions of African American resi-dents.We used this evidence to support an argu-ment that the tobacco industry was targeting African Americans for their products.Afinal example of the utility of GIS is pro-vided by Goldstein et al.(2003)in a case study of192Luke Fig.3.Targeted placement of tobacco billboards with African American images in1998.Symbols indicate type of image found on the tobacco billboard.Census block groupsare shaded according to percentage of African American residents.Taken from Luke,Esmundo,&Bloom(2000).the adoption of100%tobacco-free school policies in North Carolina.In this study the authors used key informant interviews to identify important fac-tors influencing the successful adoption of tobacco-free policies in14North Carolina school districts. Although key informants suggested that the local to-bacco economy had little direct influence on policy adoption,GIS analyses revealed that all school dis-tricts passing policies were located in counties with relatively little tobacco production(Fig.4).This map not only reveals an important pattern that can be the subject of further research,it allows dissemination of thisfinding in a way that is clear,powerful,and easy to understand.Network AnalysisSocial network analysis is a broad set of meth-ods for the systematic study of social structure (Degenne&Fors´e,2004).Network tools are based on the analysis of relational data—information about the connections among a set of actors,be theyGetting the Big Picture in Community Science:Methods that Capture Context193Fig.4.Location of14North Carolina school districts adopting100%tobacco-free policies.Taken from Goldsteinet al.(2003).persons,agencies,etc.This distinguishes network analysis from much of the rest of community sci-ence where attribute data are typically collected and analyzed.The relations can be almost any type of information about a connection between actors—friendship,recognition,money exchange,kinship,in-formation exchange,and respect.The relation can be very specifically defined,such as sharing needles among a network of drug users.Network analysis has been broadly used in the physical,social,and health work meth-ods have been used to model disease transmission (Rothenberg et al.,1998),job-seeking(Granovetter, 1973),the diffusion of innovations(Valente,1995), inter-organizational behavior(Mintz&Schwartz, 1981),social capital and community development (Lin,2000),and teen smoking(Alexander,Piazza, Mekos,&Valente,2001),to name just a few.The use of network analysis in community sci-ence grew out of the work on social support.So-cial support was thought to have a positive effect on a wide variety of important individual character-istics and behavior,including coping,bereavement, suicide,general physical and mental health(Cohen,Underwood,&Gottlieb,2000).Early on,social sup-port tended to be measured by simply asking indi-viduals to rate how much support they received from others.A network analysis approach to social sup-port became attractive as community scientists dis-tinguished between perceived social support and the relational or structural aspects of social support.That is,researchers began to‘move beyond the individ-ual’(Felton&Shinn,1992)and see that social sup-port was not just a psychological characteristic but an outcome of being embedded in a social network of friends,family,and other support providers.We started seeing studies that had participants identifying their own support networks.This allowed investigators to determine the size of the supportive networks,distinguish support by source,and even get some very simple measures of network structure.For example,in an innovative study of social support sat-isfaction,Stokes(1983)had each participant identify up to20people who were important in their lives and with whom they had monthly contact.After identify-ing the network member,each participant drew lines connecting each pair of people in their lists who were significant in each other’s lives.This allowed Stokes。

相关文档
最新文档